首页> 外文OA文献 >Socially shared metacognitive regulation during collaborative learning processes in student dyads and small groups
【2h】

Socially shared metacognitive regulation during collaborative learning processes in student dyads and small groups

机译:学生组和小组中的协作学习过程中的社交共享元认知调控

代理获取
本网站仅为用户提供外文OA文献查询和代理获取服务,本网站没有原文。下单后我们将采用程序或人工为您竭诚获取高质量的原文,但由于OA文献来源多样且变更频繁,仍可能出现获取不到、文献不完整或与标题不符等情况,如果获取不到我们将提供退款服务。请知悉。

摘要

Traditionally metacognition has been theorised, methodologically studied and empirically tested from the standpoint mainly of individuals and their learning contexts. In this dissertation the emergence of metacognition is analysed more broadly. The aim of the dissertation was to explore socially shared metacognitive regulation (SSMR) as part of collaborative learning processes taking place in student dyads and small learning groups. The specific aims were to extend the concept of individual metacognition to SSMR, to develop methods to capture and analyse SSMR and to validate the usefulness of the concept of SSMR in two different learning contexts; in face-to-face student dyads solving mathematical word problems and also in small groups taking part in inquiry-based science learning in an asynchronous computer-supported collaborative learning (CSCL) environment. This dissertation is comprised of four studies. In Study I, the main aim was to explore if and how metacognition emerges during problem solving in student dyads and then to develop a method for analysing the social level of awareness, monitoring, and regulatory processes emerging during the problem solving. Two dyads comprised of 10-year-old students who were high-achieving especially in mathematical word problem solving and reading comprehension were involved in the study. An in-depth case analysis was conducted. Data consisted of over 16 (30–45 minutes) videotaped and transcribed face-to-face sessions. The dyads solved altogether 151 mathematical word problems of different difficulty levels in a game-format learning environment. The interaction flowchart was used in the analysis to uncover socially shared metacognition. Interviews (also stimulated recall interviews) were conducted in order to obtain further information about socially shared metacognition. The findings showed the emergence of metacognition in a collaborative learning context in a way that cannot solely be explained by individual conception. The concept of socially-shared metacognition (SSMR) was proposed. The results highlighted the emergence of socially shared metacognition specifically in problems where dyads encountered challenges. Small verbal and nonverbal signals between students also triggered the emergence of socially shared metacognition. Additionally, one dyad implemented a system whereby they shared metacognitive regulation based on their strengths in learning. Overall, the findings suggested that in order to discover patterns of socially shared metacognition, it is important to investigate metacognition over time. However, it was concluded that more research on socially shared metacognition, from larger data sets, is needed. These findings formed the basis of the second study. In Study II, the specific aim was to investigate whether socially shared metacognition can be reliably identified from a large dataset of collaborative face-to-face mathematical word problem solving sessions by student dyads. We specifically examined different difficulty levels of tasks as well as the function and focus of socially shared metacognition. Furthermore, the presence of observable metacognitive experiences at the beginning of socially shared metacognition was explored. Four dyads participated in the study. Each dyad was comprised of high-achieving 10-year-old students, ranked in the top 11% of their fourth grade peers (n=393). Dyads were from the same data set as in Study I. The dyads worked face-to-face in a computer-supported, game-format learning environment. Problem-solving processes for 251 tasks at three difficulty levels taking place during 56 (30–45 minutes) lessons were video-taped and analysed. Baseline data for this study were 14 675 turns of transcribed verbal and nonverbal behaviours observed in four study dyads. The micro-level analysis illustrated how participants moved between different channels of communication (individual and interpersonal). The unit of analysis was a set of turns, referred to as an ‘episode’. The results indicated that socially shared metacognition and its function and focus, as well as the appearance of metacognitive experiences can be defined in a reliable way from a larger data set by independent coders. A comparison of the different difficulty levels of the problems suggested that in order to trigger socially shared metacognition in small groups, the problems should be more difficult, as opposed to moderately difficult or easy. Although socially shared metacognition was found in collaborative face-to-face problem solving among high-achieving student dyads, more research is needed in different contexts. This consideration created the basis of the research on socially shared metacognition in Studies III and IV. In Study III, the aim was to expand the research on SSMR from face-to-face mathematical problem solving in student dyads to inquiry-based science learning among small groups in an asynchronous computer-supported collaborative learning (CSCL) environment. The specific aims were to investigate SSMR’s evolvement and functions in a CSCL environment and to explore how SSMR emerges at different phases of the inquiry process. Finally, individual student participation in SSMR during the process was studied. An in-depth explanatory case study of one small group of four girls aged 12 years was carried out. The girls attended a class that has an entrance examination and conducts a language-enriched curriculum. The small group solved complex science problems in an asynchronous CSCL environment, participating in research-like processes of inquiry during 22 lessons (á 45–minute). Students’ network discussion were recorded in written notes (N=640) which were used as study data. A set of notes, referred to here as a ‘thread’, was used as the unit of analysis. The inter-coder agreement was regarded as substantial. The results indicated that SSMR emerges in a small group’s asynchronous CSCL inquiry process in the science domain. Hence, the results of Study III were in line with the previous Study I and Study II and revealed that metacognition cannot be reduced to the individual level alone. The findings also confirm that SSMR should be examined as a process, since SSMR can evolve during different phases and that different SSMR threads overlapped and intertwined. Although the classification of SSMR’s functions was applicable in the context of CSCL in a small group, the dominant function was different in the asynchronous CSCL inquiry in the small group in a science activity than in mathematical word problem solving among student dyads (Study II). Further, the use of different analytical methods provided complementary findings about students’ participation in SSMR. The findings suggest that it is not enough to code just a single written note or simply to examine who has the largest number of notes in the SSMR thread but also to examine the connections between the notes. As the findings of the present study are based on an in-depth analysis of a single small group, further cases were examined in Study IV, as well as looking at the SSMR’s focus, which was also studied in a face-to-face context. In Study IV, the general aim was to investigate the emergence of SSMR with a larger data set from an asynchronous CSCL inquiry process in small student groups carrying out science activities. The specific aims were to study the emergence of SSMR in the different phases of the process, students’ participation in SSMR, and the relation of SSMR’s focus to the quality of outcomes, which was not explored in previous studies. The participants were 12-year-old students from the same class as in Study III. Five small groups consisting of four students and one of five students (N=25) were involved in the study. The small groups solved ill-defined science problems in an asynchronous CSCL environment, participating in research-like processes of inquiry over a total period of 22 hours. Written notes (N=4088) detailed the network discussions of the small groups and these constituted the study data. With these notes, SSMR threads were explored. As in Study III, the thread was used as the unit of analysis. In total, 332 notes were classified as forming 41 SSMR threads. Inter-coder agreement was assessed by three coders in the different phases of the analysis and found to be reliable. Multiple methods of analysis were used. Results showed that SSMR emerged in all the asynchronous CSCL inquiry processes in the small groups. However, the findings did not reveal any significantly changing trend in the emergence of SSMR during the process. As a main trend, the number of notes included in SSMR threads differed significantly in different phases of the process and small groups differed from each other. Although student participation was seen as highly dispersed between the students, there were differences between students and small groups. Furthermore, the findings indicated that the amount of SSMR during the process or participation structure did not explain the differences in the quality of outcomes for the groups. Rather, when SSMRs were focused on understanding and procedural matters, it was associated with achieving high quality learning outcomes. In turn, when SSMRs were focused on incidental and procedural matters, it was associated with low level learning outcomes. Hence, the findings imply that the focus of any emerging SSMR is crucial to the quality of the learning outcomes. Moreover, the findings encourage the use of multiple research methods for studying SSMR. In total, the four studies convincingly indicate that a phenomenon of socially shared metacognitive regulation also exists. This means that it was possible to define the concept of SSMR theoretically, to investigate it methodologically and to validate it empirically in two different learning contexts across dyads and small groups. In-depth micro-level case analysis in Studies I and III showed the possibility to capture and analyse in detail SSMR during the collaborative process, while in Studies II and IV, the analysis validated the emergence of SSMR in larger data sets. Hence, validation was tested both between two environments and within the same environments with further cases. As a part of this dissertation, SSMR’s detailed functions and foci were revealed. Moreover, the findings showed the important role of observable metacognitive experiences as the starting point of SSMRs. It was apparent that problems dealt with by the groups should be rather difficult if SSMR is to be made clearly visible. Further, individual students’ participation was found to differ between students and groups. The multiple research methods employed revealed supplementary findings regarding SSMR. Finally, when SSMR was focused on understanding and procedural matters, this was seen to lead to higher quality learning outcomes. Socially shared metacognition regulation should therefore be taken into consideration in students’ collaborative learning at school similarly to how an individual’s metacognition is taken into account in individual learning.
机译:传统的元认知已经从个人及其学习环境的角度出发进行了理论,方法论研究和经验检验。本文对元认知的出现进行了较广泛的分析。论文的目的是探索社会共享的元认知调节(SSMR),作为在学生二元组和小型学习小组中进行的协作学习过程的一部分。具体目标是将个人元认知的概念扩展到SSMR,开发捕获和分析SSMR的方法,并验证SSMR概念在两种不同学习环境中的有用性;在面对面的学生二元组中解决数学单词问题,也参加了在异步计算机支持的协作学习(CSCL)环境中参加基于查询的科学学习的小组。本文由四项研究组成。在研究I中,主要目的是探讨在解决二元问题过程中是否以及如何出现元认知,然后开发一种方法来分析在解决问题过程中出现的意识,监测和监管过程的社会水平。这项由10岁学生组成的两人研究小组,特别是在数学单词问题解决和阅读理解方面成绩斐然。进行了深入的案例分析。数据由超过16个(30-45分钟)的录像和面对面的转录组成。在游戏形式的学习环境中,二元组共解决了151个不同难度级别的数学单词问题。分析中使用了交互流程图来发现社交共享的元认知。为了获得有关社交共享元认知的更多信息,进行了访谈(也激发了回忆访谈)。研究结果表明,在协作学习环境中元认知的出现不能以个人观念单独解释。提出了社会共享元认知(SSMR)的概念。结果强调了社会共享的元认知的出现,特别是在二元组遇到挑战的问题中。学生之间小的口头和非口头信号也触发了社交共享元认知的出现。另外,一个二分之一实施了一个系统,在该系统中,他们可以根据自己的学习优势共享元认知调节。总体而言,研究结果表明,为了发现社交共享元认知的模式,研究随时间变化的元认知非常重要。然而,得出的结论是,需要从更大的数据集上对社会共享的元认知进行更多的研究。这些发现构成了第二项研究的基础。在研究II中,特定目的是研究是否可以从学生二元组的协作式面对面数学单词问题解决会议的大型数据集中可靠地识别社交共享的元认知。我们专门研究了任务的不同难度级别以及社交共享元认知的功能和重点。此外,探索了在社交共享元认知开始时可观察到的元认知经验的存在。四个二元组参加了该研究。每个二元组均由成绩优异的10岁学生组成,在其四年级同龄人中排名前11%(n = 393)。二元组来自与研究I中相同的数据集。二元组在计算机支持的游戏格式学习环境中面对面工作。录制了录像并分析了在56个课程(30-45分钟)中针对三个难度级别进行的251个任务的问题解决过程。这项研究的基准数据是在四个研究二元组中观察到的14675轮转录的言语和非言语行为。微观分析说明了参与者如何在不同的沟通渠道(个人和人际交流)之间移动。分析单位是一组转折,称为“情节”。结果表明,社交共享的元认知及其功能和重点以及元认知经验的出现可以由独立编码者从较大的数据集中以可靠的方式定义。对问题的不同难度级别的比较表明,为了在小组中触发社交共享的元认知,与中等难度或容易程度相比,这些问题应该更加困难。尽管在成就卓著的学生二战中的协作式面对面问题解决中发现了社交共享的元认知,但在不同的情况下还需要进行更多的研究。这种考虑为研究III和IV中的社会共享元认知研究奠定了基础。研究III,目的是将SSMR的研究范围从学生二元组的面对面数学问题扩展到异步计算机支持的协作学习(CSCL)环境中的小组之间基于查询的科学学习。具体目的是调查SSMR在CSCL环境中的演变和功能,并探讨SSMR如何在查询过程的不同阶段出现。最后,研究了学生在此过程中参与SSMR的情况。进行了一个深入的解释性案例研究,该研究小组由四个12岁的小女孩组成。这些女孩参加了一次入学考试,并进行了语言丰富的课程。这个小组在异步CSCL环境中解决了复杂的科学问题,在22节课(约45分钟)内参加了类似于研究的探究过程。学生的网络讨论记录在书面笔记(N = 640)中,用作学习数据。一组注释(这里称为“线程”)用作分析单位。编码器间协议被认为是重要的。结果表明,SSMR出现在科学领域的一个小组的异步CSCL查询过程中。因此,研究III的结果与先前的研究I和研究II一致,并揭示了元认知不能仅仅降低到个人水平。这些发现还证实,应该将SSMR作为一个过程进行检查,因为SSMR可以在不同的阶段演进,并且不同的SSMR线程是重叠和交织的。尽管SSMR功能的分类适用于一小组CSCL,但在科学活动中该小组的异步CSCL查询中的主导功能与学生二元组中数学单词问题解决方法不同(研究II)。此外,使用不同的分析方法可以提供有关学生参加SSMR的补充结论。研究结果表明,仅编写单个书面注释或仅检查谁在SSMR线程中拥有最多注释,而且还检查注释之间的联系是不够的。由于本研究的结果基于对单个小组的深入分析,因此在研究IV中研究了更多病例,并研究了SSMR的研究重点,并在面对面的情况下进行了研究。 。在研究IV中,总体目标是调查在进行科学活动的小型学生团体中,来自异步CSCL查询过程的具有更大数据集的SSMR的出现。具体目的是研究在该过程的不同阶段中SSMR的出现,学生对SSMR的参与以及SSMR重点与结果质量之间的关系,这在以前的研究中并未进行探讨。参与者是与研究III相同班级的12岁学生。五个小组由四名学生和五名学生之一(N = 25)组成,参与了研究。小组在异步CSCL环境中解决了不确定的科学问题,参与了长达22小时的类似于研究的询问过程。书面笔记(N = 4088)详细介绍了小组的网络讨论,这些构成了研究数据。通过这些注释,探索了SSMR线程。与研究III中一样,线程被用作分析单位。总共332个笔记被分类为形成41个SSMR线程。三位编码员在分析的不同阶段对编码器间的协议进行了评估,发现它们是可靠的。使用了多种分析方法。结果表明,SSMR出现在小组中的所有异步CSCL查询过程中。但是,研究结果并未揭示该过程中SSMR出现的任何显着变化趋势。作为主要趋势,SSMR线程中包含的注释数量在流程的不同阶段有显着差异,并且小组之间也存在差异。尽管学生的参与程度在学生之间是高度分散的,但学生与小组之间存在差异。此外,调查结果表明,在此过程或参与结构中SSMR的数量不能解释各组结果质量的差异。相反,当SSMR专注于理解和程序问题时,它与实现高质量的学习成果相关。反过来,当SSMR专注于偶然事件和程序问题时,则与学习水平低下有关。因此研究结果表明,任何新兴SSMR的关注点对于学习成果的质量都是至关重要的。此外,研究结果鼓励使用多种研究方法来研究SSMR。总体而言,这四项研究令人信服地表明,也存在社会共享的元认知调节现象。这意味着有可能在理论上定义SSMR的概念,进行方法论的研究并在跨双胞胎和小组的两种不同的学习环境中进行经验验证。研究I和III中的深入微观案例分析显示了在协作过程中捕获和详细分析SSMR的可能性,而研究II和IV中的分析验证了SSMR在较大数据集中的出现。因此,在进一步的情况下,验证是在两个环境之间以及在相同环境中进行的。作为本文的一部分,揭示了SSMR的详细功能和焦点。此外,研究结果表明,可观察到的元认知经验作为SSMRs的起点具有重要作用。显然,如果要使SSMR清晰可见,小组要处理的问题将相当困难。此外,发现学生的个人参与在学生和团体之间是不同的。所采用的多种研究方法揭示了有关SSMR的补充发现。最后,当SSMR专注于理解和程序问题时,这被认为可以带来更高质量的学习成果。因此,在学生在学校的协作学习中应考虑社会共享的元认知规则,这与在个人学习中考虑个人元认知的方式类似。

著录项

  • 作者

    Iiskala Tuike;

  • 作者单位
  • 年度 2015
  • 总页数
  • 原文格式 PDF
  • 正文语种 en
  • 中图分类

相似文献

  • 外文文献
  • 中文文献
  • 专利

客服邮箱:kefu@zhangqiaokeyan.com

京公网安备:11010802029741号 ICP备案号:京ICP备15016152号-6 六维联合信息科技 (北京) 有限公司©版权所有
  • 客服微信

  • 服务号