...
首页> 外文期刊>Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences >Social Modulation of Circadian Synchronization and Temporal Perception in Complete Isolation
【24h】

Social Modulation of Circadian Synchronization and Temporal Perception in Complete Isolation

机译:完全隔离中昼夜同步和时间知觉的社会调节

获取原文
   

获取外文期刊封面封底 >>

       

摘要

In principle, two domains of temporal control in human behavior and cognition can be distinguished, i.e., long-term mechanisms as observed in circadian or circannual rhythms reflecting evolutionary adaptations to geophysical cycles, and self-organized mechanisms as mainly exhibited for short-term intervals in the sub-second to minutes domain. It is an open question whether the mechanisms of temporal control within and also across the temporal domains are inter-dependent, thus, reflecting one dominant temporal control machinery, or whether temporal control within and across the different temporal domains underlies independent operative principles. One way to get more insight in these problems is to manipulate environmental variables, and to investigate whether such manipulations show similar or distinct effects on temporal control; one class of such environmental variables are represented by social interactions. We describe results of an exploratory study in which insight into the problem of homogeneity or inhomogeneity in temporal processing could be gained. Four adult young male participants were isolated completely for three weeks without any access to external information; the participants did not have any knowledge about the time of day. The same participants had participated before this group experiment in an experiment with individual isolation. All participants showed during individual isolation “free-running” circadian rhythms different from 24hours. The question was in the group experiment whether the participants would socially synchronize, i.e., exhibit one circadian rhythm for the group. The rest-activity cycle was measured for each participant, and body temperature and kidney functions were monitored. In addition, participants during their subjective days had to perform several tests like the production of 10-s intervals, or they had to indicate their “personal tempo”, i.e., tapping with their fingers at a comfortable and self-selected speed. The results indicate interesting synchronizations and desynchronizations of functions. In the first half of the experiment, the rest- activity cycle of the four participants was synchronized with a circadian period of 26.2hrs. One participant, however, showed an important phase difference compared to the others starting his subjective day on average 2hrs earlier. The phase difference observed in this “critical participants” indicates that his circadian clock had a tendency for a shorter subjective day compared to the others. This could be proven by the circadian modulation of the vegetative functions as represented for instance by kidney functions. Most surprisingly, the circadian modulation of the kidney functions disappeared a few days after the beginning of the experiment, but reappeared spontaneously after several days. This suppression of the circadian modulation can be interpreted as a beat phenomenon as also observed in physical phenomena; when two near frequency sounds are superimposed they suppress each other in antiphase. The same mechanism can be assumed for our observation: The rest-activity-cycle of this participant was synchronized with the other group members, but this period was apparently too long for other physiological functions to be synchronized; they followed their own circadian cycle which was shorter. Thus, the social synchronization of the participant resulted in an internal desynchronization, which became observable through the beat phenomenon. Within this participant, two circadian clocks were running at the same time, one super-imposed by the group and one dominated by the individual circadian clock. A support for this interpretation is provided by the second half of the experiment when the “critical participant” no longer lived synchronized with the group but followed his own temporal schedule close to 24hrs, whereas the other three group members extended their circadian day in synchrony to even 27.2hrs. In this second half of the experiment, the “critical participant” no longer exhibited an internal desynchronization. To the best of our knowledge such a beat phenomenon with two superimposed circadian cycles has not been observed previously. Another unique result was obtained in experiments on temporal production. Before the experiment started each participant went through a learning phase to produce as accurately as possible a 10-s interval without counting. All participants reached within a few trials a reliable set point with only little variance. During isolation each participant selected within several days of transitions very different individual set points when producing the 10-s intervals: One participant produced the target interval as 6.5 s, another one as 8.5 s, a third one as 11 s, and the “critical participant” as 16 s. Thus, no correlation was observed between the circadian rhythms of these participants and their temporal production confirming a previous observation that temporal perception in the domain of secon
机译:从原则上讲,可以区分人类行为和认知的时间控制的两个领域,即,在昼夜节律或昼夜节律中观察到的长期机制反映了对地球物理周期的进化适应,而自组织机制主要表现为短期间隔在不到一秒到几分钟的范围内。这是一个悬而未决的问题,即时域内部以及跨时域的时间控制机制是否相互依赖,从而反映出一种主要的时域控制机制,还是不同时域内部以及跨不同时域的时间控制是否构成独立的操作原理。对这些问题有更多了解的一种方法是操纵环境变量,并调查这种操纵对时间控制是否表现出相似或不同的影响。一类此类环境变量由社会互动来表示。我们描述了一项探索性研究的结果,其中可以获得对时间处理中同质性或不均质性问题的见解。四名成年男性青年参与者被完全隔离了三个星期,没有任何外部信息;参与者对一天中的时间一无所知。在小组实验之前,相同的参与者已经参加了单独隔离的实验。所有参与者在个人孤立时都表现出不同于24小时的“自由奔跑”昼夜节律。问题在于小组实验中,参与者是否会进行社交同步,即对小组表现出一种昼夜节律。测量每个参与者的静息活动周期,并监测体温和肾功能。另外,参与者在主观的日子里必须进行几次测试,例如以10秒为间隔进行锻炼,或者他们必须表明自己的“个人节奏”,即以舒适且自选的速度敲击手指。结果表明有趣的功能同步和不同步。在实验的前半部分,四个参与者的休息活动周期与26.2小时的昼夜节律同步。然而,与其他人相比,平均开始2小时开始他的主观一天的参与者,表现出重要的相位差。在“关键参与者”中观察到的相位差表明,他的昼夜节律时钟与其他时钟相比有较短的主观一天的趋势。这可以通过对生物功能的昼夜节律调节来证明,例如由肾功能代表。最令人惊讶的是,肾脏功能的昼夜节律调节在实验开始后的几天就消失了,但在几天后自然而然地出现。昼夜节律调制的这种抑制可以解释为拍打现象,也可以在物理现象中观察到。当两个近频声音叠加在一起时,它们彼此反相抑制。我们的观察可以采用相同的机制:该参与者的休息活动周期与其他组成员同步,但是这个时间段显然太长,无法同步其他生理功能;他们遵循自己的昼夜节律周期,周期较短。因此,参与者的社交同步导致内部不同步,这种内部不同步可以通过跳动现象来观察。在该参与者中,同时运行着两个生物钟,一个由小组叠加,另一个由个体生物钟控制。当“关键参与者”不再与小组同步生活,而是按照他自己的时间表进行接近24小时的实验时,实验的后半部分为这种解释提供了支持,而其他三个小组成员则将他们的昼夜节律同步延长到即使是27.2小时在实验的后半部分,“关键参与者”不再表现出内部不同步。据我们所知,以前没有观察到具有两个重叠的昼夜节律的跳动现象。在临时生产的实验中获得了另一个独特的结果。在实验开始之前,每个参与者都要经过一个学习阶段,以尽可能准确地产生10秒的间隔,而无需计算。在少数试验中,所有参与者均达到了几乎没有差异的可靠设定点。在隔离期间,每个参与者在过渡的几天内选择产生10秒间隔的非常不同的单个设定点:一个参与者产生的目标间隔为6.5 s,另一个为8.5 s,第三个为11 s,“临界”参与者”为16 s。因此,在这些参与者的昼夜节律与他们的时间产生之间未观察到相关性,这证实了先前的观察,即secon领域中的时间知觉

著录项

相似文献

  • 外文文献
  • 中文文献
  • 专利
获取原文

客服邮箱:kefu@zhangqiaokeyan.com

京公网安备:11010802029741号 ICP备案号:京ICP备15016152号-6 六维联合信息科技 (北京) 有限公司©版权所有
  • 客服微信

  • 服务号