首页> 美国卫生研究院文献>other >Does Anyone Know the Answer to that Question? Individual Differences in Judging Answerability
【2h】

Does Anyone Know the Answer to that Question? Individual Differences in Judging Answerability

机译:有人知道这个问题的答案吗?判断答疑人的个人差异

代理获取
本网站仅为用户提供外文OA文献查询和代理获取服务,本网站没有原文。下单后我们将采用程序或人工为您竭诚获取高质量的原文,但由于OA文献来源多样且变更频繁,仍可能出现获取不到、文献不完整或与标题不符等情况,如果获取不到我们将提供退款服务。请知悉。

摘要

Occasionally people may attempt to judge whether a question can be answered today, or if not, if it can be answered in the future. For example, a person may consider whether enough is known about the dangers of living close to a nuclear plant, or to a major electricity cable, for them to be willing to do so, and state-authorities may consider whether questions about the dangers of new technologies have been answered, or in a reasonable future can be, for them to be willing to invest money in research aiming develop such technologies. A total of 476 participants, for each of 22 knowledge questions, either judged whether it was answerable today (current answerability), or judged when it could be answered (future answerability). The knowledge questions varied with respect to the expected consensus concerning their answerability: consensus questions (high expected consensus), non-consensus questions (lower expected consensus), and illusion questions (formulated to appear answerable, but with crucial information absent). The questions’ judged answerability level on the two scales was highly correlated. For both scales, consensus questions were rated more answerable than the non-consensus questions, with illusion questions falling in-between. The result for the illusion questions indicates that a feeling of answerability can be created even when it is unlikely that somebody can come up with an answer. The results also showed that individual difference variables influenced the answerability judgments. Higher levels of belief in certainty of knowledge, mankind’s knowledge, and mankind’s efficacy were related to judging the non-consensus questions as more answerable. Participants rating the illusion questions as answerable rated the other answerability questions as more, or equally, answerable compared to the other participants and showed tendencies to prefer a combination of more epistemic default processing and less intellectual processing.
机译:有时,人们可能会尝试判断问题是否可以在今天回答,如果不能在将来回答,则不能回答。例如,一个人可能会考虑是否对居住在核电站或主要电缆附近的危险有足够的了解,以使他们愿意这样做,而国家主管部门可能会考虑是否存在关于以下危险的问题:新技术已经被回答,或者在合理的未来,因为他们愿意为旨在开发此类技术的研究投资。总共476名参与者针对22个知识问题中的每一个,判断了今天是否可回答(当前的可回答性)或判断何时可以回答(未来的可回答性)。知识问题在关于其可回答性的预期共识方面有所不同:共识问题(预期期望值高),非共识性问题(预期期望值较低)和幻觉问题(拟定为看起来可回答,但缺少关键信息)。在两个量表上,问题的判断可回答性水平高度相关。对于这两个量表,共识问题的得分都比非共识问题的答案更高,而错觉问题介于两者之间。幻觉问题的结果表明,即使某人不太可能提出答案,也可以产生一种可回答的感觉。结果还表明,个体差异变量会影响回答性判断。人们对知识的确定性,人类的知识和人类的效能的更高信念与判断非共识性问题的答案有关。与其他参与者相比,参与者对错觉问题的回答为“可回答”,对其他可回答性问题的评价为“更多或同等”,并​​且显示出倾向于将更多的认知默认处理和较少的智力处理相结合的倾向。

著录项

相似文献

  • 外文文献
  • 中文文献
  • 专利
代理获取

客服邮箱:kefu@zhangqiaokeyan.com

京公网安备:11010802029741号 ICP备案号:京ICP备15016152号-6 六维联合信息科技 (北京) 有限公司©版权所有
  • 客服微信

  • 服务号