...
首页> 外文期刊>Attention, perception & psychophysics >Searching for the right word: Hybrid visual and memory search for words
【24h】

Searching for the right word: Hybrid visual and memory search for words

机译:搜索正确的单词:混合视觉和记忆搜索单词

获取原文
获取原文并翻译 | 示例
   

获取外文期刊封面封底 >>

       

摘要

In "hybrid search" (Wolfe Psychological Science, 23(7), 698-703, 2012), observers search through visual space for any of multiple targets held in memory. With photorealistic objects as the stimuli, response times (RTs) increase linearly with the visual set size and logarithmically with the memory set size, even when over 100 items are committed to memory. It is well-established that pictures of objects are particularly easy to memorize (Brady, Konkle, Alvarez, & Oliva Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 14325-14329, 2008). Would hybrid-search performance be similar if the targets were words or phrases, in which word order can be important, so that the processes of memorization might be different? In Experiment 1, observers memorized 2, 4, 8, or 16 words in four different blocks. After passing a memory test, confirming their memorization of the list, the observers searched for these words in visual displays containing two to 16 words. Replicating Wolfe (Psychological Science, 23(7), 698-703, 2012), the RTs increased linearly with the visual set size and logarithmically with the length of the word list. The word lists of Experiment 1 were random. In Experiment 2, words were drawn from phrases that observers reported knowing by heart (e.g., "London Bridge is falling down"). Observers were asked to provide four phrases, ranging in length from two words to no less than 20 words (range 21-86). All words longer than two characters from the phrase, constituted the target list. Distractor words were matched for length and frequency. Even with these strongly ordered lists, the results again replicated the curvilinear function of memory set size seen in hybrid search. One might expect to find serial position effects, perhaps reducing the RTs for the first (primacy) and/or the last (recency) members of a list (Atkinson & Shiffrin, 1968; Murdock Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 482-488, 1962). Surprisingly, we showed no reliable effects of word order. Thus, in "London Bridge is falling down," "London" and "down" were found no faster than "falling.".
机译:在“混合搜索”中(Wolfe Psychological Science,23(7),698-703,2012),观察者通过视觉空间搜索存储在存储器中的多个目标中的任何一个。使用逼真的对象作为刺激,即使将100多个项目提交给内存,响应时间(RT)也会随着视觉设置的大小线性增加,并随着存储设置的大小呈对数增长。众所周知,物体的图片特别容易记忆(Brady,Konkle,Alvarez和Oliva Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,105,14325-14329,2008)。如果目标是单词或短语(单词顺序很重要,从而记忆的过程可能会有所不同),则混合搜索的性能是否会相似?在实验1中,观察者在四个不同的块中记忆了2、4、8或16个单词。通过记忆测试后,确认记忆列表,观察者在包含2至16个单词的视觉显示中搜索了这些单词。复制Wolfe(Psychological Science,23(7),698-703,2012),RT随视觉设置的大小线性增加,而与单词列表的长度成对数关系。实验1的单词表是随机的。在实验2中,单词是从观察者表示心知肚明的短语中抽出来的(例如,“伦敦桥正在倒下”)。观察员被要求提供四个短语,长度范围从两个单词到不少于20个单词(范围21-86)。短语中所有超过两个字符的单词构成目标列表。干扰词的长度和频率相匹配。即使有了这些有序的列表,结果仍会复制在混合搜索中看到的存储集大小的曲线函数。可能希望发现序列位置的影响,也许会减少列表的第一个(首要)和/或最后一个(新近)成员的RT(Atkinson和Shiffrin,1968; Murdock Journal of Experiment Psychology,64,482-488, 1962)。令人惊讶的是,我们没有显示出单词顺序的可靠效果。因此,在“伦敦桥倒下”中,发现“伦敦”和“倒下”没有比“倒下”快。

著录项

相似文献

  • 外文文献
  • 中文文献
  • 专利
获取原文

客服邮箱:kefu@zhangqiaokeyan.com

京公网安备:11010802029741号 ICP备案号:京ICP备15016152号-6 六维联合信息科技 (北京) 有限公司©版权所有
  • 客服微信

  • 服务号