首页> 外文期刊>Frontiers in Human Neuroscience >A Response to: Commentary: Stabilizing Constructs through Collaboration across Different Research Fields as a Way to Foster the Integrative Approach of the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) Project
【24h】

A Response to: Commentary: Stabilizing Constructs through Collaboration across Different Research Fields as a Way to Foster the Integrative Approach of the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) Project

机译:回应:评论:通过跨不同研究领域的合作稳定构架,以促进研究领域标准(RDoC)项目的整合方法

获取原文

摘要

Glannon ( 2016 ) argues that mechanistic explanations are reductive and thus not good models for understanding the environmentally situated dynamic causal relationships characteristic of psychopathological phenomena. He rejects my claim that the Research Domain Criteria Project's (RDoC's) success is contingent on the collective stabilization of RDoC constructs (Sullivan, 2016 ) because he takes construct stabilization to be a requirement applying exclusively to mechanistic explanations. Here, I argue that the dynamic causal relationships Glannon describes require evidential support and construct stabilization has an important role to play in establishing these relationships. Glannon offers several illustrations of dynamic causal relationships. He explains that acute or chronic psychosocial stress can cause dysregulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, which increases cortisol levels in the brain. Excess cortisol can contribute to neuronal degeneration in brain areas like prefrontal cortex, which can have deleterious effects on cognition, mood regulation, and motivation. In support of these dynamic causal relationships, Glannon cites a research study (H?hne et al., 2014 ) that operationalizes “psychosocial stress” with an experimental paradigm known as The Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) . The TSST is “a public speaking task involving a mock job interview and mental arithmetic” (2014, p. 269). In the version of the task used in this study, “participants were given a 10-min preparation time for a presentation about their professional education,” which they had to give “in front of a mixed-gender panel of two judges” who withheld “verbal and non-verbal feedback” (2014, p. 269). I consulted this research study directly because Glannon does not explain what “psychosocial stress” is. Although the authors of the study indicate the TSST produces psychosocial stress, they define it operationally by means of the TSST. This leaves several questions unanswered: What is psychosocial stress? Are there different types? Do all types activate the same cascades of events (e.g., psychological/cognitive, cellular, molecular)? What types of phenomena are produced using the TSST? How do these phenomena compare with phenomena produced by other psychosocial stress-inducing experimental paradigms? Two exemplary meta-analyses contain some responses to these questions. Dickerson and Kemeny, for example, focused on 208 studies investigating “the effects of psychosocial stressors on cortisol activation” (Dickerson and Kemeny, 2004 , p. 355). Their motivation was that cortisol changes are not “uniformly triggered” across experimental paradigms used to induce psychosocial stress, so they wanted to determine what features set some experimental paradigms apart. They claim those experimental paradigms accompanied by the greatest increases in cortisol placed subjects in situations where they were “in danger of negative evaluation of important and valued aspects of [themselves] by others” (“social-evaluative threat”) and were unable to control situation outcome (“outcome uncontrollability”) (2004, p. 377). Dickerson and Kemeny's study is suggestive that not all experimental paradigms used to study the relationship between psychosocial stress and cortisol responses are equivalent. Insofar as task demands or type of stressors differ across experimental paradigms, it is likely that different cascades of events (e.g., psychological/cognitive, neurophysiological) are triggered when subjects are trained in them. Dickerson and Kemeny also indicate that if we use the word “stress in a vague and diffuse way” these differences may be obscured and “prevent[] focused research on specific kinds of threats that can affect health-relevant physiological systems” (Dickerson and Kemeny, 2004 , p. 377). A second meta-analysis undertaken by Frisch and colleagues addresses the question of what phenomena the TSST measures. They regard the fact that the “scope of the TSST is limited to inducing […] social-evaluative threat” (2015, p. 12) as a weakness. They also claim that because the TSST contains “two-stress-inducing-elements” namely, social evaluation and uncontrollability, it is difficult to determine whether each element has “independent effects” downstream (e.g., on psychological/cognitive processes or neurophysiological processes). Despite these weaknesses they assert that “the TSST protocol is highly standardized” and this “high degree of standardization allows for comparisons between different studies in one field of research” which “facilitates the integration of these findings in reviews and meta-analysis” and allows for “the replication and extension of previous studies” (Frisch et al., 2015 , p. 12). At best, what can be concluded on the basis of Frisch and colleagues' analysis is that the TSST may be a valuable experimental paradigm for studying the relationship between social-evaluative threats and neurophysiological stress respons
机译:Glannon(2016)认为,机械的解释是还原性的,因此不是理解心理病理现象特征的环境动态因果关系的好模型。他拒绝了我的观点,即研究领域标准项目(RDoC)的成功取决于RDoC结构的集体稳定化(Sullivan,2016),因为他将结构稳定化作为仅适用于机械解释的要求。在这里,我认为,格兰农描述的动态因果关系需要证据支持,而构筑物的稳定化在建立这些关系中起着重要作用。格兰农提供了一些动态因果关系的例证。他解释说,急性或慢性社会心理压力会导致下丘脑-垂体-肾上腺(HPA)轴失调,从而增加大脑中的皮质醇水平。过量的皮质醇可能会导致大脑额叶皮质等额叶区域的神经元变性,从而对认知,情绪调节和动机产生有害影响。为了支持这些动态的因果关系,Glannon引用了一项研究研究(H?hne等,2014),该研究通过称为“特里尔社会压力测试”(TSST)的实验范式来实现“社会心理压力”。 TSST是“涉及模拟工作面试和心算的公开演讲任务”(2014年,第269页)。在本研究中使用的任务版本中,“给参与者10分钟的准备时间来介绍他们的专业教育”,他们必须在“两位法官组成的混合性别专家小组面前”作答“语言和非语言反馈”(2014年,第269页)。我之所以直接咨询了这项研究,是因为Glannon并未解释“心理压力”是什么。尽管研究的作者指出,TSST会产生社会心理压力,但他们还是通过TSST在操作上对其进行定义。这就留下了几个问题无法回答:什么是社会心理压力?有不同的类型吗?所有类型都激活相同的事件级联(例如,心理/认知,细胞,分子)吗?使用TSST会产生哪些类型的现象?这些现象与其他由社会心理压力引起的实验范式产生的现象相比如何?两个示例性荟萃分析包含对这些问题的一些回答。例如,Dickerson和Kemeny专注于208个研究,研究“心理社会压力源对皮质醇活化的影响”(Dickerson和Kemeny,2004,第355页)。他们的动机是,在用来诱发社会心理压力的实验范式中,皮质醇的变化不是“统一触发”的,因此他们想确定哪些特征使某些实验范式与众不同。他们声称那些实验范式伴随着皮质醇放置受试者的最大增加,而这些受试者“处于他人对[自身]重要和有价值方面进行负面评价的危险”(“社会评价威胁”)并且无法控制情况结果(“结果不可控”)(2004年,第377页)。 Dickerson和Kemeny的研究表明,并非所有用于研究社会心理压力与皮质醇反应之间关系的实验范式都是等效的。就实验范式而言,任务要求或压力源类型不同,在对受试者进行培训时,可能会触发不同的事件级联(例如,心理/认知,神经生理学)。 Dickerson和Kemeny还指出,如果我们使用“以模糊和分散的方式施加压力”一词,这些差异可能会被掩盖,并且“防止[]专注于可能影响与健康相关的生理系统的特定种类威胁的研究”(Dickerson和Kemeny ,2004年,第377页)。 Frisch及其同事进行的第二次荟萃分析解决了TSST可以测量什么现象的问题。他们认为“ TSTS的范围仅限于诱发[...]社会评价威胁”这一事实(2015年,第12页)是一个弱点。他们还声称,由于TSST包含“双重压力诱导元素”,即社会评估和不可控制性,因此很难确定每个元素是否对下游都有“独立影响”(例如,对心理/认知过程或神经生理过程的影响)。 。尽管存在这些弱点,他们仍然断言“ TSST协议是高度标准化的”,并且这种“高度标准化允许在一个研究领域中进行不同研究之间的比较”,这“有助于将这些发现整合到评论和荟萃分析中”,并允许(Frisch等人,2015,第12页)。充其量,根据弗里施及其同事的分析可以得出的结论是,TSST可能是研究社会评估威胁与神经生理应激反应之间关系的有价值的实验范例。

著录项

相似文献

  • 外文文献
  • 中文文献
  • 专利
获取原文

客服邮箱:kefu@zhangqiaokeyan.com

京公网安备:11010802029741号 ICP备案号:京ICP备15016152号-6 六维联合信息科技 (北京) 有限公司©版权所有
  • 客服微信

  • 服务号