...
首页> 外文期刊>Frontiers in Neurology >Commentary: Parent-Reported Behavioral and Psychiatric Problems Mediate the Relationship between Sleep-Disordered Breathing and Cognitive Deficits in School-Aged Children
【24h】

Commentary: Parent-Reported Behavioral and Psychiatric Problems Mediate the Relationship between Sleep-Disordered Breathing and Cognitive Deficits in School-Aged Children

机译:评论:父母报告的行为和精神病学问题介导学龄儿童睡眠障碍呼吸与认知缺陷之间的关系

获取原文
   

获取外文期刊封面封底 >>

       

摘要

The association between sleep-disordered breathing (SDB) and behavior problems has been well established since 1982. Smith and colleagues attempt to expand upon this known association ( 1 ). They are to be commended for identifying an important and clinically relevant question: Why does cognitive functioning fail to improve following treatment of SDB in school-age children? However, we believe that their attempt to answer this question falls short conceptually and statistically. Their operationalization of such broad constructs as “cognitive functioning” and “behavior problems” appears to be inadequate, and their statistical modeling seems incomplete. As a result, the answer that they infer to the question that they pose—that behavior problems are the mediating factor hindering improvement in cognitive functioning after treatment of sleep disordered breathing in children—may be misconceived.First, the authors ignore the caveat emphasized by all well-known neuropsychologists regarding the appropriate use of tests and measures in clinical assessment ( 2 – 4 ). As David Wechsler, Jerome Sattler, and other luminaries in the field of neuropsychology emphasize in their definitive texts on the assessment of intellectual functioning and cognitive abilities in children and adults, single test scores and even more importantly subtest scores should never be interpreted in isolation, as they yield an incomplete and unreliable picture of individual functioning, whether cognitive, behavioral, social, or psychological. The recommended approach for evaluating any functional domain is to use multiple tests to characterize the domain, examine all test scores, look for consistent and converging patterns in the data, and interpret these patterns in the context of corroborating information provided by parents, teachers, and other sources. Using individual subtest scores to characterize complex and variable functional domains leaves too much potential for error and misinterpretation.Despite these caveats, the authors use subscale scores from only two screening measures completed by parents [Child Behavior Checklist-Revised (CBCL), Connors’ Parent Rating Scales-Revised (CPRS-R)] to characterize complex functional domains such as “problematic behaviors” and “psychiatric concerns.” Their chosen measures may be standardized, well-validated, and widely used, but they are screening measures designed to have greater sensitivity than specificity. As evidence of the appropriateness of their chosen measures, the authors cite single studies with only modest results on childhood disorders unrelated to the ones they are examining (bipolar disorder or autism spectrum disorder from citations 31 and 32) or refer to meta-analyses using scales that they do not employ in their study (CBCL-Attention Problem from citation 35). The authors also use NEPSY individual subtest scores to “capture” cognitive deficits. No matter how strong the overall psychometrics of their chosen cognitive measures, which the authors duly provide, they cannot overcome the limitations inherent in using single subtest scores to characterize entire domains of complex functioning. Because of these psychometric limitations, neither we nor the authors can be confident that the conceptual constructs they use in their analyses are adequately and validly operationalized. This is especially true when behavior and cognitive performance are highly variable and constantly changing, as is true for developing children.Second, the authors attempt to show that behavior problems mediate between SDB and cognitive functioning. Their choice of recently developed variations on structural equation modeling that allow assessment of mediators (resampling-based mediation and ratio-of-mediator-probability-weights) indicates an understanding of quantitative methods that is likely more sophisticated than ours. It is unclear, however, why the authors test only models where behavior mediates between SDB and cognitive functioning, as cognition might equally mediate between SDB and behavioral functioning. Furthermore, the correlation between SDB and cognition or behavior is low (?0.21 or 0.26, respectively Ref. ( 1 ) from Figure 1) and likely inflated by the size of the sample ( 5 ). This suggests that SDB accounts for only limited variance in either construct, raising questions about the clinical relevance or explanatory power of the model. Finally, the authors fail to measure and include important covariates, such as child IQ, parent education level, and family socioeconomic status, which can correlate even more highly with cognitive functioning and behavior problems than the covariates that they do include (age, sex, race, BMI) ( 6 ) and which might render the impact of sleep disordered breathing on behavior problems and cognitive functioning negligible. The statistical model that they offer appears to be incomplete and not well articulated, leaving the reader unconvinced that the authors have specifi
机译:自1982年以来,睡眠障碍呼吸(SDB)与行为问题之间的关联就得到了很好的确立。Smith及其同事尝试扩大这种已知的关联(1)。为识别一个重要且与临床相关的问题而受到赞扬:为什么学龄儿童接受SDB治疗后认知功能为何无法改善?但是,我们认为,他们在回答这个问题上的尝试在概念和统计上都不够。他们对诸如“认知功能”和“行为问题”之类的广泛构想的操作性似乎不足,其统计模型似乎并不完整。结果,他们推断出他们所提出的问题的答案(行为问题是阻碍儿童睡眠呼吸障碍治疗后认知功能改善的中介因素)可能会被误解。首先,作者忽略了有关在临床评估中适当使用测试和测量方法的所有知名神经心理学家(2-4)。正如David Wechsler,Jerome Sattler和其他神经心理学领域的专家在他们的权威性文章中强调了对儿童和成人的智力功能和认知能力的评估一样,单项测验分数,甚至更重要的是,子测验分数绝不能孤立地解释,因为它们会产生个体功能(认知,行为,社会或心理)的不完整且不可靠的图像。推荐的评估任何功能领域的方法是使用多个测试来表征该领域,检查所有测试分数,在数据中寻找一致且趋同的模式,并在父母,教师和家长提供的信息得到证实的情况下解释这些模式。其他来源。尽管使用了这些警告,但作者使用了单独的子测验分数来表征复杂和可变的功能域,但存在太多的错误和误解的可能性。作者仅使用了由父母完成的两种筛查方法得到的子测验分数[儿童行为清单修订版(CBCL),康纳斯的父母修订量表(CPRS-R)]以表征复杂的功能域,例如“问题行为”和“精神病问题”。他们选择的措施可能是标准化的,经过良好验证的并被广泛使用,但它们是筛选措施,旨在提高敏感性而不是特异性。为了证明所选择措施的适当性,作者引用了单项研究,但对与他们所检查的疾病无关的儿童疾病(双极性疾病或自引31和32的自闭症谱系障碍)仅进行了适度的研究,或使用量表引用了荟萃分析他们没有在研究中使用的内容(CBCL-注意问题来自引文35)。作者还使用NEPSY个人子测验分数来“捕捉”认知缺陷。无论作者适当提供的所选认知测度的整体心理测验多么强大,他们都无法克服使用单个子测验分数来表征复杂功能的整个领域所固有的局限性。由于这些心理测验的局限性,我们和作者都无法确信他们在分析中使用的概念构造已被充分有效地实施。当行为和认知表现高度可变且不断变化时,尤其是这样,对于发育中的儿童而言,情况尤其如此。其次,作者试图证明行为问题在SDB和认知功能之间介导。他们选择了最近开发的结构方程模型变体,以评估介体(基于重采样的介体和介体概率权重比),表明他们对定量方法的理解可能比我们的更为复杂。但是,尚不清楚为什么作者只测试行为在SDB和认知功能之间介导的模型,因为认知可能在SDB和行为功能之间平等地介导。此外,SDB与认知或行为之间的相关性很低(分别为0.21或0.26,分别来自图1的参考文献(1)),并且可能因样本的大小而膨胀(5)。这表明,SDB仅解释了两种结构的有限差异,从而引发了有关模型的临床相关性或解释力的问题。最后,作者未能衡量和纳入重要的协变量,例如儿童智商,父母的受教育程度和家庭社会经济状况,与他们所包含的协变量(年龄,性别, (BMI)(6),这可能使睡眠呼吸障碍对行为问题和认知功能的影响可以忽略不计。他们提供的统计模型似乎不完整,缺乏清晰的表述,这使读者不敢相信作者已经指定了

著录项

相似文献

  • 外文文献
  • 中文文献
  • 专利
获取原文

客服邮箱:kefu@zhangqiaokeyan.com

京公网安备:11010802029741号 ICP备案号:京ICP备15016152号-6 六维联合信息科技 (北京) 有限公司©版权所有
  • 客服微信

  • 服务号