首页> 外文OA文献 >Snelle aanpassingen van het gangpatroon; balansverstoringen bij jongeren en ouderen
【2h】

Snelle aanpassingen van het gangpatroon; balansverstoringen bij jongeren en ouderen

机译:快速调整步态模式;平衡年轻人和老年人的困扰

摘要

While we walk around, busy with our daily routines, we rarely consider how complex our gait needs to be in order to handle all the irregularities of our environment. We walk on uneven curbs, avoid puddles of water, pets, running children etc., almost without thinking about it. Even if we lose balance and trip over something, we are often able to recover. This ability deteriorates with aging, when falls become a prominent problem, but one should keep in mind that, if we were unable to adjust our ongoing steps when faced with sudden changes in our environment, falls would occur at any age. Although adjusting step trajectories is crucial for our ability to navigate the environment, it is not known under which circumstances or how this can be accomplished. Therefore, the ability to adjust leg movements during ongoing gait, either perturbed or unperturbed, is the focus of this thesis.In general, our bodies move by executing motor commands. For example, we decide to ‘walk to the sofa’ and send this command to our executing elements (i.e., our legs). At the same time the motor command is sent to the legs, its copy is sent to a neural controller called the internal model. The internal model predicts what kind of movement is expected and monitors feedback information from our body and new information from the environment. If there is a mismatch between our movement and what is appropriate and expected, our internal model can determine a movement adjustment is needed. For example, if a cat runs in your path while you are walking towards your sofa, your internal model should act to trigger an adjustment of your step so that you do not step on it. Depending on the situation, sometimes a simple online correction of the ongoing movement is sufficient and sometimes the movement needs to be replaced by a whole new motor plan. In case of walking, our leg swing trajectories are planned in advance based on the visual information from the environment, our goals etc. Therefore, if we encounter a change in the environment our pre-planned step needs to be stopped before an alternative foot landing position can be found.The objective of this thesis was to provide insights into the ability of humans to adjust leg movements during ongoing gait, both unperturbed and perturbed. The background and rationale for this thesis are described in Chapter 1. Until now, adjustments of ongoing movements were mostly investigated using arm movements or simple leg movements like step initiation, but gait is more challenging, since it is an ongoing movement that poses considerable balance constraints. Furthermore, it is one of the most common daily life activities requiring leg movement adjustments. Therefore, insights into the ability to adjust ongoing gait, both unperturbed and perturbed, might provide useful information in the context of fall prevention and rehabilitation of various patient populations.In the first part of this thesis we focused on unperturbed gait and developed novel walking tasks in which subjects had to walk on a treadmill by following virtual stepping stones projected onto the treadmill’s surface. These stepping stones could change during the approach, forcing the subjects to adjust their precisely aimed steps. This method is described in Chapter 2.In our first experiment, described in Chapter 3, the stepping stones could change color suddenly, which was an indication they became obstacles to avoid. Therefore, subjects had to adjust their ongoing steps to land outside the obstacle in any way they preferred. Using this task we were able to show that response inhibition (i.e., the ability to stop a movement) plays an important role in obstacle avoidance and we could see that older adults performed worse than young. However, unlike young, older adults also showed learning effects and improved with practice. Furthermore, we paired the walking task with a cognitive task that required inhibition, and found that the performance of both groups deteriorated when the two tasks were performed simultaneously. However, the two groups handled this problem differently and only older adults prioritized their performance on the walking task. Finally, we could see that the difficulties older adults experienced were related to response inhibition, since their performance deteriorated specifically when inhibition was required.In the second experiment (Chapter 4), instead of changing color, the stepping stones could shift position and subjects had to adjust their steps to follow the stepping stone. Unlike the previous task, this forced subjects to adjust their movements in a specific direction and we found that the direction of stepping stone displacement influenced the accuracy of movement adjustments in young adults. Adjustments were most accurate when step lengthening was required and least accurate for step shortening. Furthermore, the difference in accuracy between step lengthening and shortening became smaller with increasing time pressure. This difference in accuracy suggests a higher risk of unsuccessfully executing a leg movement adjustment when a step is being shortened as opposed to lengthened. Since both step shortening and lengthening are viable options for obstacle avoidance, this difference in risk might affect the way obstacle avoidance strategies are chosen.In the second part of the thesis we focused on a condition even more challenging than unperturbed gait by investigating the ability to adjust leg movements during tripping. Tripping occurs frequently in our daily lives and leads to falling, unless we are able to make an appropriate recovery step that recovers balance and lands in a safe area. In the final experiment, as described in Chapter 5, we investigated whether it is possible to adjust such steps, which are already adjustments of ongoing gait in order to recover balance, and how this is accomplished. We tripped young adults and presented them with a forbidden landing zone (FZ) at trip onset. Since this FZ was positioned in the area where they would normally land following tripping, they were forced to adjust the ongoing trip recovery step in order to avoid it. All of our subjects were able to avoid landing in the FZ, but there were individual differences in performance. Some subjects succeeded already in their first trial while others improved over the course of the experiment and succeeded only in the final, fifth trial. Different strategies were used, subjects either shortened their steps or stepped to the side of the FZ. While most subjects used step shortening, shorter subjects tended to step to the size of the FZ, probably because it was positioned too close to the tripping obstacle since their trip recovery steps were shorter. Strikingly, some subjects were even able to switch between strategies. However, irrespective of the strategy used and success of FZ avoidance, balance recovery following tripping was not compromised. Furthermore, we observed strong anticipation effects and subjects adjusted their trip responses even on trials that did not involve a FZ.Finally, to describe the mechanisms driving these adjustments in Chapter 6 we analyzed muscle activity changes occurring during step shortening, the dominant FZ avoidance strategy. Step shortening was driven by muscle activity changes occurring in two functionally different stages. The first stage of muscle activity change occurred around 100 ms following trip onset, which is too early to be considered voluntary, and did not contribute to the observed step shortening. Therefore, we suggest this initial stage might have served as a ‘pause’ until an appropriate movement adjustment was initiated. Second stage of the muscle activity changes occurred at latencies corresponding to voluntary reaction and clearly led to the observed step adjustments (i.e., step shortening and landing on the toes). Interestingly, we found similar muscle activity changes on trials that did not involve a FZ, in line with step adjustments that occurred under the influence of anticipation.In conclusion, and as discussed in Chapter 7, this thesis shows that it is possible, albeit challenging, to investigate movement adjustments during gait using paradigms common in fundamental arm and eye movement research. These paradigms typically investigate online adjustments and response inhibition separately, but our work demonstrates both are involved in adjustments of gait. In general, our data show that unperturbed and perturbed gait can be modified quickly. Apparently, fast movement adjustments are not only possible for eye, arm, and simple leg movements, but even for extremely challenging whole body movements, such as balance recovery following tripping. Finally, learning effects observed in our experiments show that it is possible to improve leg movement adjustment abilities, which is promising for fall prevention, especially in light of our aging society.
机译:在我们忙于日常工作的时候四处走走时,我们很少考虑为了应对环境中的所有不规则因素,步态需要多么复杂。我们走在不平坦的路缘上,避免积水,养宠物,奔跑的孩子等,几乎不用考虑。即使我们失去平衡并绊倒了东西,我们通常也能够康复。当跌倒成为一个突出的问题时,这种能力会随着年龄的增长而恶化,但是应该牢记,如果在环境突然变化的情况下我们无法调整正在进行的步骤,则任何年龄的人都会跌倒。尽管调整步距轨迹对于我们在环境中的导航能力至关重要,但在哪种情况下或如何实现这一点尚不知道。因此,在进行中的步态时调节腿部动作的能力是本论文的重点。通常,我们的身体通过执行运动命令来运动。例如,我们决定“步行到沙发上”并将此命令发送到执行元素(即我们的腿)。同时,将电机命令发送到腿,将其副本发送到称为内部模型的神经控制器。内部模型可以预测预期的移动方式,并监视来自人体的反馈信息和来自环境的新信息。如果我们的运动与适当和预期的运动之间不匹配,我们的内部模型可以确定需要进行运动调整。例如,如果一只猫在您走向沙发时在您的路径上奔跑,则您的内部模型应采取行动来触发您的脚步调整,以免您踩到它。根据情况,有时只需对正在进行的运动进行简单的在线校正就可以了,有时还需要用全新的运动计划来代替运动。在行走的情况下,我们会根据来自环境,目标等方面的视觉信息预先计划腿部摆动的轨迹。因此,如果我们遇到环境变化,则需要停止预先计划的步骤,然后再进行另一次脚部着陆本论文的目的是提供洞察人类在持续步态过程中调整腿部运动的能力的能力,包括步态和步态。本论文的背景和基本原理在第1章中进行了描述。到目前为止,大部分的进行中的动作调整都是使用手臂动作或简单的腿部动作(如踩踏动作)来进行研究,但步态更具挑战性,因为它是一种持续进行的动作,具有相当大的平衡性。约束。此外,它是最常见的日常生活活动之一,需要调整腿部动作。因此,对调整持续的步态的能力的见解,无论是正常的还是不稳定的,都可以在各种患者的跌倒预防和康复中提供有用的信息。在本文的第一部分中,我们着重研究了平稳的步态并开发了新颖的步行任务在这种情况下,受试者必须跟随在跑步机表面上投射的虚拟垫脚石在跑步机上行走。这些垫脚石可能会在进场过程中发生变化,迫使受试者调整其精确瞄准的步伐。该方法在第2章中进行了描述。在第3章中描述的第一个实验中,垫脚石可能会突然变色,这表明它们已成为避免的障碍。因此,受试者必须调整他们正在进行的步骤,以他们喜欢的任何方式降落在障碍物之外。使用此任务,我们能够证明反应抑制(即停止运动的能力)在避障方面起着重要作用,并且我们可以看到,老年人的表现比年轻人差。但是,与年轻人不同,老年人也表现出学习效果并通过练习得到改善。此外,我们将步行任务与需要抑制的认知任务配对,发现当同时执行两项任务时,两组的表现都会下降。但是,两组对这个问题的处理方式不同,只有老年人优先考虑他们在步行任务中的表现。最后,我们可以看到老年人遇到的困难与反应抑制有关,因为他们的表现在需要抑制时会特别恶化。在第二个实验(第4章)中,垫脚石可以改变位置,而被测者可以改变位置,而不用改变颜色调整自己的脚步,跟随垫脚石。与以前的任务不同,这迫使受试者在特定方向上调整他们的运动,我们发现踏脚石位移的方向会影响年轻人的运动调整的准确性。当需要延长步​​长时,调整最准确,而缩短步长则最不准确。此外,步长和步长之间的精度差异随时间压力的增加而变小。这种准确性上的差异表明,在缩短步伐而非延长步伐的情况下,无法成功执行腿部运动调整的风险较高。由于缩短步长和延长步长都是避开障碍物的可行选择,因此这种风险差异可能会影响选择避障策略的方式。在本文的第二部分中,我们研究了一种比无障碍步态更具挑战性的状况调整绊倒时的腿部动作。跳闸在我们的日常生活中经常发生并导致跌倒,除非我们能够采取适当的恢复步骤来恢复平衡并降落在安全区域。在第5章中描述的最终实验中,我们研究了是否有可能调整这些步骤(这些步骤已经在调整步态以恢复平衡)以及如何实现。我们绊倒了年轻人,并在旅途开始时向他们展示了禁止着陆区(FZ)。由于该FZ位于跳闸后通常会降落的区域,因此他们被迫调整正在进行的跳闸恢复步骤,以避免跳闸。我们所有的受试者都能够避免进入FZ,但是在性能上存在个体差异。一些受试者在第一次试验中已经成功,而其他受试者在试验过程中有所改善,仅在最后的第五次试验中成功。使用了不同的策略,受试者要么缩短步伐,要么步入FZ一侧。尽管大多数对象使用了缩短步长,但是较短的对象倾向于步调到FZ的大小,这可能是因为由于他们的跳闸恢复步骤更短,所以其位置离跳闸障碍物太近了。令人惊讶的是,有些受试者甚至能够在策略之间进行切换。但是,无论采用何种策略以及避免FZ成功与否,跳闸后的平衡恢复都不会受到影响。此外,我们观察到了强烈的预期效果,即使在不涉及FZ的试验中,受试者也可以调整其跳闸反应。最后,为了在第6章中描述驱动这些调整的机制,我们分析了缩短步伐(FZ的主要避免策略)期间发生的肌肉活动变化。缩短步伐是由在两个功能不同阶段发生的肌肉活动变化驱动的。肌肉活动变化的第一阶段发生在旅行开始后约100毫秒,这还为时过早,不能被认为是自愿的,并且没有有助于观察到的步伐缩短。因此,我们建议在开始适当的运动调整之前,此初始阶段可能已成为“暂停”。肌肉活动的第二阶段变化发生在与自愿反应相对应的等待时间上,并明显导致观察到的步调(即步短和脚趾着地)。有趣的是,在不涉及FZ的试验中,我们发现了类似的肌肉活动变化,这与在预期的影响下发生的阶跃调整一致。总之,如第7章所述,本论文表明,尽管具有挑战性,但仍有可能,使用基本的手臂和眼睛运动研究中常见的范例来研究步态期间的运动调节。这些范例通常分别研究在线调整和反应抑制,但是我们的工作表明两者都参与了步态的调整。总的来说,我们的数据表明,不受干扰的步态和步态都可以快速改变。显然,快速运动调整不仅适用于眼睛,手臂和简单的腿部运动,甚至适用于极富挑战性的全身运动,例如绊倒后的平衡恢复。最后,在我们的实验中观察到的学习效果表明,有可能提高腿部运动的调节能力,这对于预防跌倒很有希望,尤其是考虑到我们的老龄化社会。

著录项

  • 作者

    Potocanac Zrinka;

  • 作者单位
  • 年度 2015
  • 总页数
  • 原文格式 PDF
  • 正文语种 en
  • 中图分类

相似文献

  • 外文文献
  • 中文文献
  • 专利

客服邮箱:kefu@zhangqiaokeyan.com

京公网安备:11010802029741号 ICP备案号:京ICP备15016152号-6 六维联合信息科技 (北京) 有限公司©版权所有
  • 客服微信

  • 服务号