class='head no_bottom_margin' id='sec1title'>Int'/> Tracking the Leader: Gaze Behavior in Group Interactions
首页> 美国卫生研究院文献>iScience >Tracking the Leader: Gaze Behavior in Group Interactions
【2h】

Tracking the Leader: Gaze Behavior in Group Interactions

机译:跟踪领导者:小组互动中的凝视行为

代理获取
本网站仅为用户提供外文OA文献查询和代理获取服务,本网站没有原文。下单后我们将采用程序或人工为您竭诚获取高质量的原文,但由于OA文献来源多样且变更频繁,仍可能出现获取不到、文献不完整或与标题不符等情况,如果获取不到我们将提供退款服务。请知悉。

摘要

class="head no_bottom_margin" id="sec1title">IntroductionIt is commonly believed that leadership is reflected in gaze behavior. Stereotypical thinking links leadership to prolonged gazing toward leaders () and longer mutual gazing in response to interactions initiated by leaders (). However, evidence for an actual relationship between leadership and social gaze behaviors is limited. To date, investigations on the influence of leadership on gaze behavior have focused on computer-based paradigms that do not provide any opportunity for social interaction (, , ). The aim of the present study was to develop a novel approach to investigate how leadership shapes gaze dynamics during real-world human group interactions.Authentic social situations are complex and highly dynamic (). What is more, unlike computer-based paradigms, they involve the potential for social interaction and reciprocity. When looking at a representation of a social stimulus (e.g., images of people), individuals need not worry about what their own gaze might be communicating to the stimulus. When looking at real people, in contrast, the eyes not only collect information (encoding function) but also communicate information to others (signaling function; ). This dual function of gaze yields an interdependency among multi-agent gaze patterns, which traditional computer-based paradigms, be they static or dynamic scene-viewing tasks, arguably fail to capture ().Despite a growing understanding of the necessity of studying social cognitive processes in interactive () and complex settings (), little is known about the influence of leadership on gaze-based interactions in unconstrained group interactions. Older studies report that, in dyadic interactions, attribution of power increases as the proportion of looking while speaking increases (, , ). However, the evidence is inconclusive as to whether gazing decoupled from speaking time identifies leaders (). Moreover, it remains unclear whether the same dynamics constraining dyads also constrain group interactions.A major reason for the lack of studies investigating group gaze-based interactions is the difficulty of simultaneously tracking transient variations in multi-party gaze features to capture the implicit semantics of social gaze behaviors. In the attempt to overcome these limitations, in this study, we developed a novel tripartite approach combining (1) computer vision methods for remote gaze-tracking, (2) a detailed taxonomy to encode the implicit semantics of multi-party gaze features, and (3) advance machine learning methods to establish dependencies between leadership and visual behaviors during unconstrained group interactions involving four people simultaneously. The basic idea for establishing a relationship between social gaze behavior and leadership was to conceptualize multi-party gaze features as patterns and to treat the analysis as a pattern classification problem: can a classifier applied to the visual behavior pattern of real people interacting in small groups reveal the leader? This is the first question we addressed in the study described here. The second question is whether the relationship between gaze behavior and leadership generalizes across leadership styles and situational conditions—in other words, whether gaze behavior can serve as a general marker of leadership.Drawing on ideas from social psychology (, href="#bib19" rid="bib19" class=" bibr popnode">Foels et al., 2000, href="#bib36" rid="bib36" class=" bibr popnode">Livi et al., 2008, href="#bib38" rid="bib38" class=" bibr popnode">Northouse, 2016), we analyzed gaze-based interaction dynamics in four leadership settings resulting from the orthogonal manipulation of leadership style (i.e., Democratic versus Autocratic) and situational condition (i.e., Low time-pressure versus High time-pressure). Democratic leadership is expected to be more effective under situational conditions of low time-pressure, whereas autocratic leaderships are expected to be more effective under situational conditions of high time-pressure (href="#bib17" rid="bib17" class=" bibr popnode">Fiedler, 2006, href="#bib40" rid="bib40" class=" bibr popnode">Pierro et al., 2003). The orthogonal manipulation of leadership styles and situational conditions resulted in two high-fit conditions (Democratic-Low time-pressure, Autocratic-High time-pressure) and two low-fit conditions (Democratic-High time-pressure, Autocratic-Low time-pressure) (href="/pmc/articles/PMC6562365/figure/fig1/" target="figure" class="fig-table-link figpopup" rid-figpopup="fig1" rid-ob="ob-fig1" co-legend-rid="lgnd_fig1">Figure 1A; see also href="#mmc1" rid="mmc1" class=" supplementary-material">Supplemental Information and href="#mmc1" rid="mmc1" class=" supplementary-material">Figure S1 for group composition and manipulation checks). Each group, composed of one designated leader and three followers, was assigned a survival task to solve within a limited time (see href="/pmc/articles/PMC6562365/figure/fig1/" target="figure" class="fig-table-link figpopup" rid-figpopup="fig1" rid-ob="ob-fig1" co-legend-rid="lgnd_fig1">Figure 1B for the experimental setting). First, using a method for automatically estimating the Visual Focus of Attention (VFOA; href="#bib1" rid="bib1" class=" bibr popnode">Ba and Odobez, 2006, href="#bib5" rid="bib5" class=" bibr popnode">Beyan et al., 2016, href="#bib22" rid="bib22" class=" bibr popnode">Gatica-Perez, 2009, href="#bib44" rid="bib44" class=" bibr popnode">Stiefelhagen et al., 1999), we determined “who looked at whom.” Then, we established a detailed taxonomy of multi-party gaze behaviors and, combining the VFOA of individual group-members, reconstructed the gaze-based interaction dynamics. Next, we probed the actual association between leadership and gaze patterns by asking whether a pattern classification algorithm could discriminate leaders and followers among the group-members. After finding evidence for leadership classification, we finally tested whether the classifier was able to generalize across leadership styles, situational conditions, and time.href="/pmc/articles/PMC6562365/figure/fig1/" target="figure" rid-figpopup="fig1" rid-ob="ob-fig1">class="inline_block ts_canvas" href="/core/lw/2.0/html/tileshop_pmc/tileshop_pmc_inline.html?title=Click%20on%20image%20to%20zoom&p=PMC3&id=6562365_gr1.jpg" target="tileshopwindow">target="object" href="/pmc/articles/PMC6562365/figure/fig1/?report=objectonly">Open in a separate windowclass="figpopup" href="/pmc/articles/PMC6562365/figure/fig1/" target="figure" rid-figpopup="fig1" rid-ob="ob-fig1">Figure 1Study Design and Experimental Setting(A) Study design and manipulation of leadership style and situational condition.(B) Schematic reproduction of the experimental setting (drawing not to scale). Participants seated on four equidistant chairs (1), while four individual video-cameras were recording the upper part of their bodies (2).
机译:<!-fig ft0-> <!-fig @ position =“ anchor” mode =文章f4-> <!-fig mode =“ anchred” f5-> <!-fig / graphic | fig / alternatives / graphic mode =“ anchored” m1-> class =“ head no_bottom_margin” id =“ sec1title”>简介人们通常认为领导能力反映在凝视行为中。刻板的思维将领导力与对领导者的长时间凝视联系起来,并根据领导者发起的互动而更长久地相互凝视。但是,有关领导力与社交注视行为之间实际关系的证据有限。迄今为止,有关领导力对凝视行为影响的研究主要集中在基于计算机的范例上,这些范例无法为社交互动提供任何机会(,,)。本研究的目的是开发一种新颖的方法来研究在现实的人类群体互动过程中领导者如何塑造凝视动态。真实的社会情况是复杂且高度动态的()。而且,与基于计算机的范式不同,它们涉及社交互动和互惠的潜力。当查看社交刺激的表示形式(例如人物图像)时,个人不必担心自己的凝视可能与刺激传达了什么内容。相反,当看着真实的人时,眼睛不仅收集信息(编码功能),而且还与其他人交流信息(信号功能;)。注视的这种双重功能在多主体注视模式之间产生了相互依赖性,传统的基于计算机的范例(无论是静态还是动态的场景查看任务)可能无法捕获()尽管对研究社会认知的必要性有了越来越多的了解。交互()和复杂环境()中的过程,对于不受约束的群体交互中的领导力对基于凝视的交互的影响知之甚少。较早的研究表明,在二元互动中,说话时旁观的比例增加时,力量的归属也会增加(,,)。但是,关于注视时间与讲话时间脱节是否能确定领导者,尚无定论。此外,目前尚不清楚相同的动力学是否会限制双性体是否也限制了群体互动。缺乏研究基于群体注视的互动的研究的主要原因是难以同时跟踪多方注视特征中的瞬态变化以捕获隐含的语义。社会注视行为。为了克服这些限制,在本研究中,我们开发了一种新颖的三方方法,该方法结合了(1)用于远程凝视跟踪的计算机视觉方法,(2)详细的分类法以编码多方凝视特征的隐含语义,以及(3)先进的机器学习方法,可在同时涉及四个人的无限制小组互动中建立领导力与视觉行为之间的依存关系。建立社交注视行为和领导力之间关系的基本思想是将多方注视特征概念化为模式,并将分析视为模式分类问题:分类器能否应用于在小组中互动的真实人的视觉行为模式揭示领导者?这是我们在此处描述的研究中解决的第一个问题。第二个问题是,凝视行为与领导力之间的关系是否能在领导风格和情境中得到概括,换句话说,凝视行为是否可以作为领导力的一般标志。借鉴社会心理学的思想(,href =“# bib19“ rid =” bib19“ class =” bibr popnode“>燃料等,2000 ,href="#bib36" rid="bib36" class=" bibr popnode"> Livi等, 2008 ,href="#bib38" rid="bib38" class=" bibr popnode"> Northouse,2016 ),我们分析了由正交产生的四种领导力环境中基于凝视的互动动态操纵领导风格(即民主与专制)和情境条件(即低时间压力与高时间压力)。民主领导预计将在时间压力较小的情况下更有效,而独裁领导者将在时间压力较高的情况下更有效(href =“#bib17” rid =“ bib17” class = “ bibr popnode”> Fiedler,2006 ,href="#bib40" rid="bib40" class=" bibr popnode"> Pierro等人,2003 )。领导风格和情境条件的正交操纵导致两个高拟合条件(民主低时间压力,专制高时间压力)和两个低拟合条件(民主高时间压力),Autocratic-Low时间压力)(href =“ / pmc / articles / PMC6562365 / figure / fig1 /” target =“ figure” class =“ fig-table-link figpopup” rid-figpopup =“ fig1” rid- ob =“ ob-fig1” co-legend-rid =“ lgnd_fig1”>图1 A;另请参见href="#mmc1" rid="mmc1" class="Supplementary-material">补充信息和href="#mmc1" rid="mmc1" class="Supplementary-material">图S1 (用于组组成和操作检查)。每个小组由一个指定的领导者和三个跟随者组成,被分配了一项生存任务,以在有限的时间内解决(请参阅href =“ / pmc / articles / PMC6562365 / figure / fig1 /” target =“ figure” class =“图1 B(用于实验设置),如图1 B所示。首先,使用一种自动估计视觉关注焦点的方法(VFOA; href="#bib1" rid="bib1" class=" bibr popnode"> Ba和Odobez,2006 ,href = “#bib5” rid =“ bib5” class =“ bibr popnode”> Beyan等人,2016 ,href="#bib22" rid="bib22" class=" bibr popnode"> Gatica-Perez ,2009 ,href="#bib44" rid="bib44" class=" bibr popnode"> Stiefelhagen等,1999 ),我们确定了“谁在看谁。”然后,我们建立了一个多方注视行为的详细分类法,并结合单个小组成员的VFOA,重构了基于注视的交互动力学。接下来,我们通过询问模式分类算法是否可以区分组成员中的领导者和跟随者,来探究领导者和注视模式之间的实际关联。在找到领导力分类的证据之后,我们最终测试了分类器是否能够跨领导风格,情境条件和时间进行概括。<!-fig ft0-> <!-fig mode = f1-> href =“ / pmc / articles / PMC6562365 / figure / fig1 /” target =“ figure” rid-figpopup =“ fig1” rid-ob =“ ob-fig1”> <!-fig / graphic | fig / alternatives / graphic mode =“ anchored” m1-> class =“ inline_block ts_canvas” href =“ / core / lw / 2.0 / html / tileshop_pmc / tileshop_pmc_inline.html?title = Click%20on%20image%20to%20zoom&p = PMC3&id = 6562365_gr1 .jpg“ target =” tileshopwindow“> target="object" href="/pmc/articles/PMC6562365/figure/fig1/?report=objectonly">在单独的窗口中打开 < / a> class="figpopup" href="/pmc/articles/PMC6562365/figure/fig1/" target="figure" rid-figpopup="fig1" rid-ob="ob-fig1">图1 < / a> <!-标题a7->研究设计和实验环境(A)研究领导风格和情境的设计和操纵。(B)实验环境的示意图复制g(未按比例绘制)。参与者坐在四把等距的椅子上(1),而四台单独的摄像机正在记录他们身体的上半部分(2)。

著录项

相似文献

  • 外文文献
  • 中文文献
  • 专利
代理获取

客服邮箱:kefu@zhangqiaokeyan.com

京公网安备:11010802029741号 ICP备案号:京ICP备15016152号-6 六维联合信息科技 (北京) 有限公司©版权所有
  • 客服微信

  • 服务号