首页> 外文OA文献 >The psychosocial factors influencing aggressive driving behaviour
【2h】

The psychosocial factors influencing aggressive driving behaviour

机译:影响积极驾驶行为的社会心理因素

摘要

Many drivers in highly motorised countries believe that aggressive driving is increasing. While the prevalence of the behaviour is difficult to reliably identify, the consequences of on-road aggression can be severe, with extreme cases resulting in property damage, injury and even death. This research program was undertaken to explore the nature of aggressive driving from within the framework of relevant psychological theory in order to enhance our understanding of the behaviour and to inform the development of relevant interventions.udTo guide the research a provisional ‘working’ definition of aggressive driving was proposed encapsulating the recurrent characteristics of the behaviour cited in the literature. The definition was: “aggressive driving is any on-road behaviour adopted by a driver that is intended to cause physical or psychological harm to another road user and is associated with feelings of frustration, anger or threat”. Two main theoretical perspectives informed the program of research. The first was Shinar’s (1998) frustration-aggression model, which identifies both the person-related and situational characteristics that contribute to aggressive driving, as well as proposing that aggressive behaviours can serve either an ‘instrumental’ or ‘hostile’ function. The second main perspective was Anderson and Bushman’s (2002) General Aggression Model. In contrast to Shinar’s model, the General Aggression Model reflects a broader perspective on human aggression that facilitates a more comprehensive examination of the emotional and cognitive aspects of aggressive behaviour.udStudy One (n = 48) examined aggressive driving behaviour from the perspective of young drivers as an at-risk group and involved conducting six focus groups, with eight participants in each. Qualitative analyses identified multiple situational and person-related factors that contribute to on-road aggression. Consistent with human aggression theory, examination of self-reported experiences of aggressive driving identified key psychological elements and processes that are experienced during on-road aggression. Participants cited several emotions experienced during an on-road incident: annoyance, frustration, anger, threat and excitement. Findings also suggest that off-road generated stress may transfer to the on-road environment, at times having severe consequences including crash involvement. Young drivers also appeared quick to experience negative attributions about the other driver, some having additional thoughts of taking action. Additionally, the results showed little difference between males and females in the severity of behavioural responses they were prepared to adopt, although females appeared more likely to displace their negative emotions. Following the self-reported on-road incident, evidence was also found of a post-event influence, with females being more likely to experience ongoing emotional effects after the event. This finding was evidenced by ruminating thoughts or distraction from tasks. However, the impact of such a post-event influence on later behaviours or interpersonal interactions appears to be minimal.udStudy Two involved the quantitative analysis of n = 926 surveys completed by a wide age range of drivers from across Queensland. The study aimed to explore the relationships between the theoretical components of aggressive driving that were identified in the literature review, and refined based on the findings of Study One. Regression analyses were used to examine participant emotional, cognitive and behavioural responses to two differing on-road scenarios whilst exploring the proposed theoretical framework. A number of socio-demographic, state and trait person-related variables such as age, pre-study emotions, trait aggression and problem-solving style were found to predict the likelihood of a negative emotional response such as frustration, anger, perceived threat, negative attributions and the likelihood of adopting either an instrumental or hostile behaviour in response to Scenarios One and Two. Complex relationships were found to exist between the variables, however, they were interpretable based on the literature review findings. Factor analysis revealed evidence supporting Shinar’s (1998) dichotomous description of on-road aggressive behaviours as being instrumental or hostile. udThe second stage of Study Two used logistic regression to examine the factors that predicted the potentially hostile aggressive drivers (n = 88) within the sample. These drivers were those who indicated a preparedness to engage in direct acts of interpersonal aggression on the road. Young, male drivers 17–24 years of age were more likely to be classified as potentially hostile aggressive drivers. Young drivers (17–24 years) also scored significantly higher than other drivers on all subscales of the Aggression Questionnaire (Buss & Perry, 1992) and on the ‘negative problem orientation’ and ‘impulsive careless style’ subscales of the Social Problem Solving Inventory – Revised (D’Zurilla, Nezu & Maydeu-Olivares, 2002). The potentially hostile aggressive drivers were also significantly more likely to engage in speeding and drink/drug driving behaviour. With regard to the emotional, cognitive and behavioural variables examined, the potentially hostile aggressive driver group also scored significantly higher than the ‘other driver’ group on most variables examined in the proposed theoretical framework. The variables contained in the framework of aggressive driving reliably distinguished potentially hostile aggressive drivers from other drivers (Nagalkerke R2 = .39). udStudy Three used a case study approach to conduct an in-depth examination of the psychosocial characteristics of n = 10 (9 males and 1 female) self-confessed hostile aggressive drivers. The self-confessed hostile aggressive drivers were aged 24–55 years of age. A large proportion of these drivers reported a Year 10 education or better and average–above average incomes. As a group, the drivers reported committing a number of speeding and unlicensed driving offences in the past three years and extensive histories of violations outside of this period. Considerable evidence was also found of exposure to a range of developmental risk factors for aggression that may have contributed to the driver’s on-road expression of aggression. These drivers scored significantly higher on the Aggression Questionnaire subscales and Social Problem Solving Inventory Revised subscales, ‘negative problem orientation’ and ‘impulsive/careless style’, than the general sample of drivers included in Study Two. The hostile aggressive driver also scored significantly higher on the Barrett Impulsivity Scale – 11 (Patton, Stanford & Barratt, 1995) measure of impulsivity than a male ‘inmate’, or female ‘general psychiatric’ comparison group. Using the Carlson Psychological Survey (Carlson, 1982), the self-confessed hostile aggressive drivers scored equal or higher scores than the comparison group of incarcerated individuals on the subscale measures of chemical abuse, thought disturbance, anti-social tendencies and self-depreciation. Using the Carlson Psychological Survey personality profiles, seven participants were profiled ‘markedly anti-social’, two were profiled ‘negative-explosive’ and one was profiled as ‘self-centred’. udQualitative analysis of the ten case study self-reports of on-road hostile aggression revealed a similar range of on-road situational factors to those identified in the literature review and Study One. Six of the case studies reported off-road generated stress that they believed contributed to the episodes of aggressive driving they recalled. Intense ‘anger’ or ‘rage’ were most frequently used to describe the emotions experienced in response to the perceived provocation. Less frequently ‘excitement’ and ‘fear’ were cited as relevant emotions. Notably, five of the case studies experienced difficulty articulating their emotions, suggesting emotional difficulties. Consistent with Study Two, these drivers reported negative attributions and most had thoughts of aggressive actions they would like to take. Similarly, these drivers adopted both instrumental and hostile aggressive behaviours during the self-reported incident. Nine participants showed little or no remorse for their behaviour and these drivers also appeared to exhibit low levels of personal insight. Interestingly, few incidents were brought to the attention of the authorities. Further, examination of the person-related characteristics of these drivers indicated that they may be more likely to have come from difficult or dysfunctional backgrounds and to have a history of anti-social behaviours on and off the road. udThe research program has several key theoretical implications. While many of the findings supported Shinar’s (1998) frustration-aggression model, two key areas of difference emerged. Firstly, aggressive driving behaviour does not always appear to be frustration driven, but can also be driven by feelings of excitation (consistent with the tenets of the General Aggression Model). Secondly, while the findings supported a distinction being made between instrumental and hostile aggressive behaviours, the characteristics of these two types of behaviours require more examination. For example, Shinar (1998) proposes that a driver will adopt an instrumental aggressive behaviour when their progress is impeded if it allows them to achieve their immediate goals (e.g. reaching their destination as quickly as possible); whereas they will engage in hostile aggressive behaviour if their path to their goal is blocked. However, the current results question this assertion, since many of the hostile aggressive drivers studied appeared prepared to engage in hostile acts irrespective of whether their goal was blocked or not. In fact, their behaviour appeared to be characterised by a preparedness to abandon their immediate goals (even if for a short period of time) in order to express their aggression. udThe use of the General Aggression Model enabled an examination of the three components of the ‘present internal state’ comprising emotions, cognitions and arousal and how these influence the likelihood of a person responding aggressively to an on-road situation. This provided a detailed insight into both the cognitive and emotional aspects of aggressive driving that have important implications for the design of relevant countermeasures. For example, the findings highlighted the potential value of utilising Cognitive Behavioural Therapy with aggressive drivers, particularly the more hostile offenders. Similarly, educational efforts need to be mindful of the way that person-related factors appear to influence one’s perception of another driver’s behaviour as aggressive or benign. Those drivers with a predisposition for aggression were more likely to perceive aggression or ‘wrong doing’ in an ambiguous on-road situation and respond with instrumental and/or hostile behaviour, highlighting the importance of perceptual processes in aggressive driving behaviour.
机译:高度机动化的国家中的许多驾驶员都认为积极驾驶在增加。虽然很难准确地识别出这种行为的普遍性,但道路侵略的后果却很严重,极端情况下会导致财产损失,伤害甚至死亡。这项研究计划旨在从相关心理学理论的框架内探索积极驾驶的本质,以增强我们对行为的理解并为相关干预措施的发展提供信息。 ud以指导研究的临时“有效”定义提出了主动驾驶以封装文献中引用的行为的周期性特征。定义是:“侵略性驾驶是指驾驶员采取的旨在对另一名道路使用者造成身体或心理伤害并与沮丧,愤怒或威胁感相关的任何道路行为”。两个主要的理论观点为研究计划提供了依据。第一个是Shinar(1998)的挫败性侵略模型,该模型可以识别与激进驾驶有关的与人相关和与情况有关的特征,并提出激进行为可以起到“仪表性”或“敌对性”的作用。第二个主要观点是安德森和布什曼(2002)的“总侵略模型”。与Shinar的模型相反,“一般性侵略”模型反映了人类侵略的更广阔视角,有助于更全面地研究攻击性行为的情感和认知方面。 ud研究之一(n = 48)从年轻的角度研究了攻击性驾驶行为司机作为一个高风险小组,涉及进行六个焦点小组,每个小组八个参与者。定性分析确定了多种情况和与人有关的因素,这些因素会导致道路上的侵略行为。与人类侵略理论相一致,对自我报告的攻击性驾驶经历的检查确定了在道路侵略过程中经历的关键心理因素和过程。与会者列举了公路事故中的几种情感:烦恼,沮丧,愤怒,威胁和兴奋。研究结果还表明,越野产生的应力可能会转移到公路环境中,有时会造成严重后果,包括撞车事故。年轻的驾驶员似乎也很快就对其他驾驶员产生了负面的归因,其中一些人还有采取行动的其他想法。此外,结果显示,尽管女性似乎更可能取代其负面情绪,但男性和女性在准备采取的行为反应的严重程度方面差异不大。在自我报告的公路事故之后,还发现了事后影响的证据,事件发生后,女性更有可能经历持续的情绪影响。反复思考或分心于任务证明了这一发现。但是,此类事后影响对以后的行为或人际交往的影响似乎很小。 ud研究二涉及对昆士兰州不同年龄段的驾驶员完成的n = 926个调查进行定量分析。该研究旨在探讨在文献综述中确定并根据研究一的发现进行完善的激进驾驶理论成分之间的关​​系。在探索拟议的理论框架的同时,使用回归分析来检查参与者对两种不同道路情景的情绪,认知和行为反应。发现许多社会人口统计学,与状态和​​特质相关的人变量,例如年龄,研究前的情绪,特质的攻击性和解决问题的方式,可以预测负面情绪反应的可能性,例如沮丧,愤怒,感知到的威胁,负面归因,以及针对情景一和情景二采取工具性或敌对行为的可能性。发现变量之间存在复杂的关系,但是,基于文献综述的发现,它们是可以解释的。因子分析揭示了证据,这些证据支持Shinar(1998)对公路攻击行为是工具性或敌对性的二分法描述。 ud研究二的第二阶段使用逻辑回归分析来检验预测样本中潜在敌对攻击性驱动因素(n = 88)的因素。这些司机表示愿意在路上进行直接的人际侵略行为。 17-24岁的年轻男性驾驶员更有可能被归类为具有潜在敌意的好斗驾驶员。在“侵略问卷”(Buss&Perry)的所有子等级中,年轻驾驶员(17-24岁)的得分也明显高于其他驾驶员(1992年)和“社会问题解决清单”的“消极问题取向”和“冲动粗心风格”子量表(D'Zurilla,Nezu和Maydeu-Olivares,2002年)。潜在敌对的好斗驾驶员也极有可能从事超速驾驶和酒后驾驶行为。在所研究的情绪,认知和行为变量方面,在拟议的理论框架中研究的大多数变量中,潜在敌对的激进驾驶员组的得分也明显高于“其他驾驶员”组。积极驾驶框架中包含的变量可靠地将潜在敌对的积极驾驶者与其他驾驶者区分开(Nagalkerke R2 = 0.39)。研究三采用案例研究的方法,对n = 10(9名男性和1名女性)自认敌对的激进驾驶员的社会心理特征进行了深入研究。自认敌对的好斗的驾驶员年龄在24-55岁之间。这些司机中有很大一部分报告说他们接受了10年或更高的教育,并且平均收入高于平均水平。作为一个整体,驾驶员报告在过去三年中犯下了许多超速驾驶和无证驾驶违法行为,并且在此期间之外有广泛的违规历史。还发现了大量证据表明,他们面临一系列侵略性发展风险因素,这些因素可能有助于驾驶员在道路上表现出侵略性。这些参与者在“侵略性问卷”和“解决社会问题库存量表”,“消极问题倾向”和“冲动/粗心风格”方面的得分明显高于研究二中的总体得分。在Barrett冲动量表– 11(Patton,Stanford&Barratt,1995)的冲动量度上,敌对的积极驾驶员的得分也明显高于男性“囚犯”或女性“一般精神病学”比较组。使用卡尔森心理调查(卡尔森,1982年),在化学滥用,思想障碍,反社会倾向和自我贬值的次级量表上,自认敌对的积极进取者的得分与被监禁者的比较组相同或更高。使用卡尔森心理调查的人格特征,有7名参与者被描述为“明显反社会”,其中2人被描述为“负爆炸性”,其中一名被描述为“以自我为中心”。 ud对十个道路敌对攻击的自我报告进行定性分析,发现与文献综述和研究一中发现的情况类似的道路情况因素。其中六个案例研究报告了越野产生的压力,他们认为这是导致他们回忆起激进驾驶事件的原因。强烈的“愤怒”或“愤怒”最常用于描述因察觉到的挑衅而经历的情绪。较少将“兴奋”和“恐惧”称为相关情绪。值得注意的是,其中五个案例研究遇到了表达自己情绪的困难,表明情绪上的困难。与研究二一致,这些驱动因素报告为归因为负,并且大多数人想到了他们想采取的积极行动。同样,这些司机在自我报告的事件中采取了有工具的和敌对的攻击行为。九名参与者对自己的行为表示little悔,甚至没有悔恨,而这些驱动力也表现出较低的个人见识。有趣的是,很少有事件引起当局注意。此外,对这些驾驶员的与人相关的特征的检查表明,他们可能更可能来自于困难或功能失调的背景,并且在旅途中和旅途中都有反社会行为的历史。 ud该研究计划具有几个关键的理论含义。尽管许多发现都支持Shinar(1998)的挫折-攻击模型,但出现了两个关键的差异领域。首先,侵略性驾驶行为并不一定总是由挫败感驱动,而是也可以由兴奋感驱动(与一般侵略模型的宗旨一致)。其次,虽然这些发现支持在工具性和敌对的攻击行为之间进行区分,但这两类行为的特征需要更多的研究。例如,Shinar(1998)提出,如果驾驶员的进步阻碍了他们的近期目标(例如,尽快到达目的地),他们将采取一种工具性的侵略性行为。而如果他们通往目标的道路受到阻拦,他们就会进行敌对的侵略行为。但是,目前的结果质疑了这一说法。,因为研究的许多敌对攻击性驾驶员似乎都准备从事敌对行为,无论他们的目标是否受到阻拦。实际上,他们的行为表现为准备放弃他们的近期目标(即使是很短的时间)以表达他们的侵略性。 ud使用“总侵略模型”可以检查“当前内部状态”的三个组成部分,包括情绪,认知和唤醒,以及这些因素如何影响一个人对路况做出积极反应的可能性。这提供了对积极驾驶的认知和情感方面的详细见解,这些方面对设计相关的对策具有重要意义。例如,研究结果突显了在积极的驾驶员(尤其是较有敌意的罪犯)中使用认知行为疗法的潜在价值。同样,教育工作也应注意与人相关的因素似乎会影响一个人对另一位驾驶员的进取或良性行为的看法。那些有侵略性的驾驶员更有可能在模棱两可的道路情况下察觉到侵略或“做错事”,并以工具性和/或敌对性行为做出反应,从而突出了感知过程在激进驾驶行为中的重要性。

著录项

  • 作者

    OBrien Sharon Rosemary;

  • 作者单位
  • 年度 2011
  • 总页数
  • 原文格式 PDF
  • 正文语种 {"code":"en","name":"English","id":9}
  • 中图分类

相似文献

  • 外文文献
  • 中文文献
  • 专利

客服邮箱:kefu@zhangqiaokeyan.com

京公网安备:11010802029741号 ICP备案号:京ICP备15016152号-6 六维联合信息科技 (北京) 有限公司©版权所有
  • 客服微信

  • 服务号