...
首页> 外文期刊>Ear and hearing. >Spatial acuity in 2-to-3-year-old children with normal acoustic hearing, unilateral cochlear implants, and bilateral cochlear implants
【24h】

Spatial acuity in 2-to-3-year-old children with normal acoustic hearing, unilateral cochlear implants, and bilateral cochlear implants

机译:听力正常,单侧人工耳蜗和双侧人工耳蜗的2至3岁儿童的空间敏锐度

获取原文
获取原文并翻译 | 示例
           

摘要

OBJECTIVES:: To measure spatial acuity on a right-left discrimination task in 2-to-3-year-old children who use a unilateral cochlear implant (UCI) or bilateral cochlear implants (BICIs); to test the hypothesis that BICI users perform significantly better when they use two CIs than when using a single CI, and that they perform better than the children in the UCI group; to determine how well children with CIs perform compared with children who have normal acoustic hearing (NH); to determine the effect of intensity roving on spatial acuity. DESIGN:: Three groups of children between 26 and 36 months of age participated in this study: 8 children with NH (mean age: 30.9 months), 12 children who use a UCI (mean age: 31.9 months), and 27 children who use BICIs (mean age: 30.7 months). Testing was conducted in a large sound-treated booth with loudspeakers positioned in a horizontal arc with a radius of 1.2 m. The observer-based psychophysical procedure was used to measure the children's ability to identify the hemifield containing the sound source (right versus left). Two methods were used for quantifying spatial acuity, an adaptive-tracking method and a fixed-angle method. In Experiment 1 an adaptive tracking algorithm was used to vary source angle, and the minimum audible angle (MAA), the smallest angle at which right-left discrimination performance is better than chance, was estimated. All three groups participated in Experiment 1. In Experiment 2 source angles were fixed at ±50 degrees, and performance was evaluated by computing the number of SDs above chance. Children in the UCI and BICI groups participated in Experiment 2. RESULTS:: In Experiment 1, when stimulus intensity was roved by 8 dB, MAA thresholds were 3.3 degrees to 30.2 degrees (mean = 14.5 degrees) and 5.7 degrees to 69.6 degrees (mean = 30.9 degrees) in the NH group and in the BICI group, respectively. When the intensity level was fixed for the BICI group, performance did not improve. Within the BICI group, 5 out of 27 children obtained MAA thresholds within one SD of their peers who have NH; all five had >12 months of bilateral listening experience. In Experiment 2, BICIs provided some advantages when the intensity level was fixed. First, the BICI group outperformed the UCI group. Second, children in the BICI group who repeated the task with their 1st CI alone had statistically significantly better performance when using both devices. In addition, when intensity roving was introduced, a larger percentage of children who had 12 or more months of BICI experience continued to perform above chance than children who had <12 months of BICI experience. Taken together, the results suggest that children with BICIs have spatial acuity that is better than when using their first CI alone and than that of their peers who use UCIs. In addition, longer durations of BICI use tend to result in better performance, although this cannot be generalized to all participants. CONCLUSION:: This report is consistent with a growing body of evidence that spatial-hearing skills can emerge in young children who use BICIs. The observation that these skills are not concomitantly emerging in age- and experience-matched children who use UCIs suggests that BICIs provide cues that are necessary for these spatial-hearing skills that UCIs do not provide.
机译:目的:测量使用单侧人工耳蜗(UCI)或双侧人工耳蜗(BICI)的2至3岁儿童的左右分辨力的空间敏锐度;测试以下假设:BICI用户使用两个配置项比使用单个配置项时的性能要好得多,并且其性能要比UCI组中的儿童更好。确定具有CI的儿童与正常听觉(NH)的儿童相比表现如何;确定强度粗纱对空间敏锐度的影响。设计::三组年龄在26到36个月之间的儿童参加了这项研究:8例NH儿童(平均年龄:30.9个月),12例使用UCI的孩子(平均年龄:31.9个月)和27例使用UCI的孩子BICI(平均年龄:30.7个月)。测试是在一个大型的经过声音处理的小房间中进行的,扬声器放置在水平圆弧中,半径为1.2 m。基于观察者的心理物理程序被用来测量儿童识别包含声源的半场的能力(右与左)。两种方法用于量化空间灵敏度,一种是自适应跟踪方法,另一种是固定角度方法。在实验1中,使用自适应跟踪算法来改变源角度,并估计最小可听角度(MAA),即左右分辨性能优于偶然性的最小角度。这三组均参加了实验1。在实验2中,源角固定为±50度,并通过计算高于机会的SD数量来评估性能。 UCI和BICI组的儿童参加了实验2。结果::在实验1中,当刺激强度提高8 dB时,MAA阈值分别为3.3度至30.2度(平均值= 14.5度)和5.7度至69.6度(平均值)。 NH组和BICI组分别为30.9度)。当BICI组的强度水平固定时,性能没有改善。在BICI组中,有27名儿童中有5名在有NH的同龄人的一个SD内达到了MAA阈值;这五个人都有超过12个月的双边收听经验。在实验2中,当强度级别固定时,BICI提供了一些优势。首先,BICI集团的表现优于UCI集团。其次,BICI组中仅使用第一个CI重复执行任务的孩子,在使用两种设备时均具有统计学上显着更好的性能。另外,当采用强度粗纱时,具有BICI经验12个月或以上的儿童比具有<12个月BICI经验的儿童继续表现出更高的机会。两者合计,结果表明,与仅使用第一个CI的儿童和使用UCI的同龄儿童相比,BICI的儿童具有更高的空间敏锐度。此外,BICI使用时间越长,往往会导致性能越好,尽管不能将其推广到所有参与者。结论:该报告与越来越多的证据表明,在使用BICI的幼儿中会出现空间听觉技能。在使用UCI的年龄和经验匹配的儿童中,这些技能并没有随之出现的观察结果表明,BICI提供了UCI不提供的这些空间听觉技能所必需的线索。

著录项

相似文献

  • 外文文献
  • 中文文献
  • 专利
获取原文

客服邮箱:kefu@zhangqiaokeyan.com

京公网安备:11010802029741号 ICP备案号:京ICP备15016152号-6 六维联合信息科技 (北京) 有限公司©版权所有
  • 客服微信

  • 服务号