...
首页> 外文期刊>Ear and hearing. >Effects of complexity of visual distracters on attention and information processing speed reflected in auditory P300
【24h】

Effects of complexity of visual distracters on attention and information processing speed reflected in auditory P300

机译:视觉干扰器的复杂性对听觉P300中反映的注意力和信息处理速度的影响

获取原文
获取原文并翻译 | 示例

摘要

Objectives: The primary aim of this study was to assess the effects of continuous, visual distracters that change systematically in complexity on P300 responses generated by an auditory oddball paradigm. In addition, the type of attention given to the visual distracter was explored. It was hypothesized that P300 amplitudes would be smallest, latencies longest, and changes in scalp distribution greatest when the most complex visual distracter requiring active attention was used versus the simple visual distracter requiring passive attention. Design: Auditory-evoked P300s were collected in a sample of 10 healthy adults (mean age = 19.90 years) using a two-toned oddball paradigm (1500 Hz "frequent" tone, probability of occurrence: 0.88, and 2000 Hz "rare" target tone, probability of occurrence: 0.12). The oddball paradigm was paired with three different visual distracters, increasing in complexity. The simplest was a black fixation cross on a white background that participants were asked to view passively as they performed the auditory task of counting the target stimulus. The second visual distracter increased in complexity by introducing color and motion, as tan and medium pink squares were alternated on the screen. Participants had to actively attend to the alternating squares by looking for a hidden text message while simultaneously counting the target auditory stimulus. The third visual distracter condition increased complexity again by introducing not only color and motion, but also biological relevance as participants viewed a mouth producing nonsense syllables. Participants had to actively attend to the moving mouth to determine what it was producing while simultaneously counting the rare auditory stimulus. Results: The two more complex visual distracters that required active attention caused reductions in auditory-evoked P300 amplitudes relative to those recorded while the participants passively viewed a fixation cross. P300 amplitudes were similar whether the two more complex visual distracters (squares versus mouth) were used. P300 latencies and scalp distribution were not influenced by complexity of, or type of attention to, the visual distracter. Regardless of distracter condition, P300 amplitude was significantly smaller and P300 latency was significantly shorter at frontal sites when compared with central and parietal sites. Conclusions: Findings indicate that endogenous attentional resource allocation abilities can be effectively monitored through the addition of a complex, visually distracting task to a "classic" auditory P300 paradigm. Biological relevance of the distracting task does not seem to have an effect on the event-related potentials measured in this study, suggesting other aspects, such as whether or not a stimulus contains color or motion, may determine the efficiency of the distracter. Last, by increasing the complexity of, and amount of attention to, a visual distracter while evoking P300s using auditory stimuli, the cognitive load for the normal, healthy listener seems to be increased and the response amplitude subsequently reduced. Evoking P300s under similar conditions from disordered populations with subtle cognitive deficits (e.g., mild traumatic brain injury) may allow for increased diagnostic specificity and sensitivity over that found for P300s to classic, auditory oddball paradigms alone.
机译:目标:这项研究的主要目的是评估连续的,视觉上分散注意力的系统性变化对听觉奇异球范例产生的P300反应的影响。此外,还探讨了对视觉分散器的注意类型。假设当使用需要主动注意的最复杂视力分散器而不是需要被动注意的简单视力分散器时,P300振幅最小,延迟最长,头皮分布变化最大。设计:使用两色调的奇球范例(1500 Hz“频繁”声调,出现概率:0.88,2000 Hz“罕见”目标),在10位健康成年人(平均年龄= 19.90岁)的样本中收集听觉诱发的P300。色调,出现的可能性:0.12)。奇怪的范例与三个不同的视觉干扰物配对使用,从而增加了复杂性。最简单的是在白色背景上的黑色注视十字架,要求参与者在执行计算目标刺激的听觉任务时被动地观看。第二种视觉干扰器通过引入颜色和运动来增加复杂度,因为棕褐色和中等粉红色的正方形在屏幕上交替出现。参与者必须在寻找隐藏的文字信息的同时积极参与交替广场,同时对目标听觉刺激进行计数。当参与者看到嘴巴产生无意义的音节时,第三种视觉干扰因素不仅通过引入颜色和动作,而且还引入了生物学相关性,再次增加了复杂性。参与者必须积极注意活动的嘴,以确定它产生的声音,同时计数罕见的听觉刺激。结果:与参与者被动观看注视交叉时记录的相比,需要主动注意的两个更复杂的视觉分散器导致听觉诱发的P300振幅降低。无论是否使用了两个更复杂的视觉干扰器(正方形与嘴巴),P300振幅都相似。 P300的潜伏期和头皮分布不受视觉干扰物的复杂性或注意力类型的影响。无论干扰因素如何,与中央和顶叶部位相比,额叶部位的P300振幅均明显较小,P300潜伏期明显缩短。结论:结果表明,可以通过向“经典”听觉P300范例中添加复杂的,视觉分散注意力的任务来有效地监控内源性注意力资源分配能力。分心任务的生物学相关性似乎对本研究中测量的与事件相关的电位没有影响,表明其他方面,例如刺激物是否包含颜色或运动,可能决定分心器的效率。最后,在使用听觉刺激唤起P300时,通过增加视觉干扰器的复杂性和关注度,似乎增加了正常,健康听众的认知负担,并随后降低了反应幅度。在类似的情况下,从具有轻微认知缺陷(例如轻度脑外伤)的失调人群中唤起P300,可以使诊断特异性和敏感性比P300对单独的经典听觉奇异球范式更高。

著录项

相似文献

  • 外文文献
  • 中文文献
  • 专利
获取原文

客服邮箱:kefu@zhangqiaokeyan.com

京公网安备:11010802029741号 ICP备案号:京ICP备15016152号-6 六维联合信息科技 (北京) 有限公司©版权所有
  • 客服微信

  • 服务号