首页> 外文期刊>Experimental Brain Research >Human stance control beyond steady state response and inverted pendulum simplification
【24h】

Human stance control beyond steady state response and inverted pendulum simplification

机译:超越稳态响应和简化倒立摆的人体姿态控制

获取原文
获取原文并翻译 | 示例
           

摘要

Systems theory analyses have suggested that human upright stance can be modelled in terms of continuous multi-sensory feedback control. So far, these analyses have considered mainly steady-state responses to periodic stimuli and relied on a simplifying model of the body’s mechanics in the form of an inverted pendulum. Therefore, they may have ignored relevant aspects of the postural behaviour. To prove a more general validity of a stance control model that we previously derived from such analyses, we now presented subjects with static–dynamic stimulus combinations and assessed response transients, anterior–posterior (a–p) response asymmetries, and possible deviations from the ‘inverted pendulum’ simplification (by measuring hip and knee bending). We presented normal subjects (Ns) and vestibular loss patients (Ps) with a–p support surface tilt on a motion platform under the instruction to maintain, with eyes closed, the body upright in space. In addition, subjects were to indicate perceived platform tilt with the help of pointers. We combined a fixed-amplitude sinusoidal tilt (0.1 Hz) with static tilts that were varied in amplitude and direction. We recorded upper body (shoulder) and lower body (hip) excursions in space and centre of pressure (COP) shift, and calculated the centre of mass (COM) angular excursion. We found that: (1) Immediately prior to stimulus onset (which was highly predictable), subjects showed a small anticipatory forward lean. (2) The subsequent transient response consisted of two parts. First, the body was moved along with the platform tilt and then, in the second part, the body excursion was braked by starting tilt compensation. Upon increasing tilt amplitude, the braking point showed a pronounced saturation with for-aft asymmetry. (3) During the following prolonged tilt, the tonic body excursions saturated with increasing static tilt amplitude. This saturation also showed a for-aft asymmetry (backwards saturation more pronounced). In contrast, the dynamic body excursions did not depend on the static tilt stimulus. (4) Tilt compensation occurred mainly in the ankle joints, but also involved small synergistic bendings in hips and knees in fixed register to the ankle rotation. (5) After the end of the stimulus, the body returned towards primary position, followed by a pronounced and slowly decaying tonic overshoot which was mainly related to tilt amplitude and initial tonic body excursion. (6) The responses of Ps qualitatively resembled those of Ns, apart from larger body excursions, less pronounced saturations, and less for-aft asymmetries. (7) Perceived platform tilt of Ns and Ps was correlated with their postural tilt compensations, but unlike the postural responses the perceptual responses overestimated actual static and dynamic tilt by a factor of 3–4. Our findings suggest two, so far undescribed and highly nonlinear mechanisms in human stance control. (a) The braking during the transient response appears to reflect a ‘sensory reweighting switch’ by which subjects change from a control that is referenced to the support to one that is referenced to space. (b) The saturation of the tonic body excursion also reflects a sensory reweighting mechanism; by this, subjects keep their balancing within a certain excursion limit. The two mechanisms were originally not predicted by our stance control model, but do not invalidate it, because they can simply be added to it. Also the observed for-aft asymmetries can be accounted for (by making thresholds in the two mechanisms asymmetric). In its extended form, the model mimics the previous and the new findings. We also conclude that the ‘inverted pendulum’ simplification is a legitimate simplification. We demonstrate the utility of the model by implementing it into a humanoid robot that then mimics closely the human experimental data. Finally, we present a hypothetical concept on sensory reweighting mechanisms in human stance control, which is meant to serve as a framework for future research.
机译:系统理论分析表明,可以根据连续的多感觉反馈控制对人体直立姿势进行建模。到目前为止,这些分析主要考虑了对周期性刺激的稳态响应,并依赖于倒立摆形式的简化人体力学模型。因此,他们可能忽略了姿势行为的相关方面。为了证明我们以前从这种分析中得出的姿态控制模型的更一般的有效性,我们现在为受试者提供静态-动态刺激组合以及评估的反应瞬态,前后-(a-p)反应不对称性以及可能的偏离。 “倒立摆”简化(通过测量臀部和膝盖的弯曲度)。我们根据指令在运动平台上为a-p支撑表面倾斜的正常受试者(Ns)和前庭缺失患者(Ps)保持眼睛在身体中保持直立的空间。另外,受试者将在指示器的帮助下指示感知到的平台倾斜。我们将固定幅度的正弦曲线倾斜度(0.1 Hz)与幅度和方向变化的静态倾斜度组合在一起。我们记录了空间的上半身(肩膀)和下半身(臀部)偏移以及压力中心(COP)的偏移,并计算了质心(COM)角度偏移。我们发现:(1)在刺激发作开始之前(这是高度可预测的),受试者表现出较小的预期前倾。 (2)随后的瞬态响应包括两个部分。首先,车身随平台倾斜一起移动,然后,在第二部分中,通过开始倾斜补偿来制动车身偏移。随着倾斜幅度的增加,制动点表现出明显的饱和度,前后不对称。 (3)在随后的长时间倾斜过程中,随着静态倾斜幅度的增加,张力体的偏移逐渐饱和。此饱和度还显示出前后不对称性(向后饱和度更加明显)。相反,动态的身体偏移并不取决于静态的倾斜刺激。 (4)倾斜补偿主要发生在踝关节,但还包括髋部和膝盖的小协同弯曲,与踝关节旋转成固定位置。 (5)刺激结束后,身体回到原始位置,随后出现明显且缓慢衰减的滋补过冲,这主要与倾斜幅度和最初的滋补体偏移有关。 (6)Ps的响应在质量上与Ns相似,除了较大的身体偏移,较不明显的饱和度和较小的前后不对称性。 (7)Ns和Ps的感知平台倾斜度与其姿势倾斜度补偿相关,但与姿势响应不同,知觉响应将实际静态和动态倾斜度高估了3-4倍。我们的发现表明,在人类姿态控制中,存在两种迄今为止尚未描述且高度非线性的机制。 (a)瞬态响应期间的制动似乎反映了一种“感官重加权开关”,通过该开关,受试者可以从参考支撑的控件更改为参考空间的控件。 (b)滋补体偏移的饱和也反映了一种感觉上的加权机制;这样,主体就可以在一定的偏移范围内保持平衡。这两种机制最初不是由我们的姿势控制模型预测的,但不会使它无效,因为可以将它们简单地添加到其中。还可以考虑观察到的前后不对称性(通过使两种机制中的阈值不对称)。在扩展形式中,该模型模仿了以前和新的发现。我们还得出结论,“倒立摆”简化是合理的简化。我们通过将模型实现到人形机器人中来演示该模型的实用性,然后该机器人模仿人类的实验数据。最后,我们提出了一种关于人类姿势控制中的感觉加权机制的假想概念,旨在作为未来研究的框架。

著录项

相似文献

  • 外文文献
  • 中文文献
  • 专利
获取原文

客服邮箱:kefu@zhangqiaokeyan.com

京公网安备:11010802029741号 ICP备案号:京ICP备15016152号-6 六维联合信息科技 (北京) 有限公司©版权所有
  • 客服微信

  • 服务号