首页> 外文期刊>BMC Pediatrics >Paths to language development in at risk children: a qualitative comparative analysis (QCA)
【24h】

Paths to language development in at risk children: a qualitative comparative analysis (QCA)

机译:风险儿童语言发展的路径:一个定性对比分析(QCA)

获取原文
           

摘要

BackgroundLanguage use in children, the development of understanding and expression of words, grammar and discourse is one of the key and most complex developmental skills acquired through childhood, with far reaching affects through life. Good language development in children is important for effective long term academic, social and economic participation in society [1,2,3]. Poor language outcomes are evident from school entry [4, 5] and influenced by social determinants of health such as socioeconomic status (SES) and maternal education [6]. For this reason, language development is a focus of both public health and early childhood policy and practice [7].Language development and at risk childrenDifficulties in language and communication place a burden on the child, parent and society both financially [8] and in quality of life [9]. One in five children have poor language development at 4?years [10] and there is a social gradient to the prevalence of language difficulties [11]. Children experiencing social disadvantage (as defined by census based measures of disadvantage e.g. income, suburb, home ownership, parental employment) are twice as likely as other children to have communication difficulties at entry to school [12] and language difficulties are part of both the cause and consequence of long term disadvantage. For some children, language difficulties lead to a sequelae of cascading negative effects such as poor literacy and social participation, increased academic failure, disengagement from school, instances of juvenile incarceration, a variety of mental health difficulties, generally poorer health, reduced employment and/ or increased relationship breakdown in adulthood [2, 3, 13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21]. Often these outcomes are clustered in children who come from low socioeconomic households and have parents who present with risks which result in these children being exposed to more difficult circumstances in their childhood [22]. This group we will refer to as children who are at risk. At risk here means an exposure to a combination of risk factors that have been shown to affect child development such as: low socioeconomic status, limited resources parental capacity and /or physical needs such as housing, experiencing mental health and drug and alcohol difficulties in the home, child maltreatment and domestic violence amongst other stressors and threats [23].Risk and protective factors for language developmentUtilising a bioecological model of development [24], research has established some of the key child, maternal and environmental influences on language development in the early years. These include (but not exclusively) maternal factors such as education [25], mental health [26,27,28] and responsivity [29], a family history of communication difficulties [30]; child factors such as birth weight [31], toddler development [32] and gender [33] and environmental factors such as being read to from an early age [34, 35], numbers of children in the home [36,37,38], attending sufficient good quality childcare [39] and SES [40,41,42]. Debate continues as to whether these factors are mediators or causal in language outcomes [43].Though we know these risk and protective factors impact language development, longitudinal cohort studies have consistently shown individual factors on their own represent only small amounts of the variability in language skills [30, 44,45,46]. It has been found there is a compounding effect of multiple risk factors on vocabulary development [47]. However, how these risk and protective factors combine and impact on each other in language development has been less studied. In a recent example, Baydar and colleagues (2014) investigated the impact of a combination of multiple maternal and environmental family factors on vocabulary development in Turkish children. The responsivity of low SES mothers supported children’s vocabulary development only when the mothers were not depressed. Investigations of risk “clusters” for language development have also emerged. In a longitudinal study of Australian children, those with a risk profile related to speaking a language other than English made fast gains in language through the school years if they had few other risks. However, if they had a number of risks, both the English and non-English speaking children performed poorly. Those with many risks for poor language at the end of the study performed poorest. [47]However, not all children with risks end up with language difficulties. What protects against poor language has received some limited attention and reveals some key factors. Turkish children’s vocabulary development was protected in families of depressed mothers who were economically distressed if they were surrounded by a supportive family and community [27]. Other studies of population and impaired cohorts have found being regularly read to, attending early childhood education, participating in play and the child’s prosocial sk
机译:背景包括儿童的使用,理解和表达的语言,语法和话语的发展是通过童年获得的关键和最复杂的发展技能之一,远远达到生命。儿童的良好语言开发对于有效的长期学术,社会和经济参与社会是重要的[1,2,3]。从学校入学[4,5]中的语言结果不佳,并受到社会经济地位(SES)和产妇教育的社会决定因素的影响[6]。出于这个原因,语言发展是公共卫生和幼儿政策和实践的焦点[7]。语言发展和危险儿童的语言和沟通的危险地对儿童,父母和社会的负担在经济上[8]和生活质量[9]。五分之一的孩子在4个孩子的语言开发中有差,年份[10],语言困难的普遍存在的社会渐变[11]。遇到社会劣势的儿童(由基于人口普查的劣势措施,郊区,雇佣权,家长就业)是其他儿童在进入学校进行沟通困难的可能性是其他儿童的两倍[12],语言困难是两者的一部分长期缺点的原因与后果。对于一些孩子来说,语言困难导致级联的负面影响,如易识别和社会参与,学术失败增加,学校的脱离,少年监禁的情况下,各种精神健康困难,普遍较差,就业较差,或/或增加成年期的关系分解[2,3,13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21]。这些结果通常是来自来自低社会经济家庭的儿童,并让父母呈现出导致这些孩子在童年中暴露在更困难的情况下的风险[22]。这组我们将参考有风险的儿童。在风险下,这里的危险因素的组合是普及的,这些危险因素被证明会影响儿童发展,例如:低社会经济地位,资源有限的资源父母能力和/或住房,体验心理健康和药物和酒精困难家庭,儿童虐待和家庭暴力在其他压力源和威胁中[23]。严重和保护因素的语言发育融合的生物学模式[24],研究已经建立了一些关键的孩子,孕产妇和环境影响的语言发展早些年。这些包括(但不是完全)的母体因素,如教育[25],心理健康[26,27,28]和响应性[29],沟通困难的家族史[30];诸如出生体重[31],蹒跚学步的发展[32]和性别[33]和环境因素,如读取到休息时间,家庭中的儿童数量[34,35],如[34,35],[36,37,38] ],参加足够的优质育儿[39]和SES [40,41,42]。辩论仍在继续这些因素是否是语言结果的调解员或因果关系[43]虽然我们知道这些风险和保护因素影响语言发展,但纵向队列研究始终如一地展示了他们自己的个体因素,只有少量语言的可变性技能[30,445,46]。已经发现有多种风险因素对词汇发展的复合效应[47]。然而,这些风险和保护因素如何在语言开发中彼此相结合和影响。在最近的一个例子中,Baydar及其同事(2014年)调查了多种孕产妇和环境因素组合对土耳其儿童词汇发展的影响。低SES母亲的响应性只有当母亲没有沮丧时才支持儿童的词汇发展。还出现了风险“群集”的风险调查。在澳大利亚儿童的纵向研究中,那些风险简介有关的风险简介与英语除了英语之外的语言,如果他们有很少的其他风险,通过学年通过学年开始迅速获得。但是,如果他们有很多风险,英语和非英语的儿童都表现不佳。学习结束时语言差的风险有很多风险的人表现更贫穷。 [47]然而,并非所有风险的儿童都最终引起语言困难。针对贫困语言的保护已经收到了一些有限的关注并揭示了一些关键因素。土耳其儿童的词汇发展受到沮丧母亲家庭的保护,如果他们被支持性的家庭和社区包围,那么经济上遭受苦恼[27]。人口和受损的群组的其他研究已经发现定期读到,参加幼儿教育,参加比赛和孩子的女性化的SK

著录项

相似文献

  • 外文文献
  • 中文文献
  • 专利
获取原文

客服邮箱:kefu@zhangqiaokeyan.com

京公网安备:11010802029741号 ICP备案号:京ICP备15016152号-6 六维联合信息科技 (北京) 有限公司©版权所有
  • 客服微信

  • 服务号