首页> 外文期刊>Frontiers in Human Neuroscience >Corrigendum: Different Neural Correlates of Emotion-Label Words and Emotion-Laden Words: An ERP Study
【24h】

Corrigendum: Different Neural Correlates of Emotion-Label Words and Emotion-Laden Words: An ERP Study

机译:更正:带有情感标签的单词和带有情感的单词的神经相关性:ERP研究

获取原文
       

摘要

In the original article, there was an error. At page 4, we wrote wrong electrodes, the centrals sites should be C1/C2, C3/C4, C5/C6 instead of CP1/CP2, CP3/CP4, and CP5/CP6, making it coherent with other parts of the paper.A correction has been made to Method, EEG Recording and Processing, Paragraph 2: Based on the visual inspection grand-averaged waveforms (Figure 2), brain topography (Figure 3), and prior investigations (Zhang et al., 2014 ; Chen et al., 2015 ), we analyzed three obvious ERP components including P100, N170, and LPC within the time windows of 90–140, 140–200, and 470–620, respectively. For P100 and N170, the mean amplitude of electrodes in the occipital area (P7/P8, P9/P10, and PO7/PO8) along both hemispheres were calculated, in alignment with previous studies (Scott et al., 2009 ; Bayer et al., 2012 ). In addition, the mean amplitude of the central cites (C1/C2, C3/P4, and C5/C6) within the 470–620 ms time window (LPC) was analyzed for LPC. Statistical significance was performed through a Greenhouse-Geisser adjustment at the level of 0.05.In the original article, there was an error. At page 4, we indicated the wrong Figure with text, the brain topography should be Figure 3, while grand-averaged waveforms should be Figure 2. In the text, we used the opposite reference.A correction has been made to Method, EEG Recording and Processing, Paragraph 2: Based on the visual inspection of the grand-averaged waveforms ( Figure 2 ), brain topography ( Figure 3 ), and prior investigations (Zhang et al., 2014 ; Chen et al., 2015 ), we analyzed three obvious ERP components including P100, N170, and LPC within the time windows of 90–140, 140–200, and 470–620, respectively. For P100 and N170, the mean amplitude of electrodes in the occipital area (P7/P8, P9/P10, and PO7/PO8) along both hemispheres were calculated, in alignment with previous studies (Scott et al., 2009 ; Bayer et al., 2012 ). In addition, the mean amplitude of the central cites (C1/C2, C3/P4, and C5/C6) within the 470–620 ms time window (LPC) was analyzed for LPC. Statistical significance was performed through a Greenhouse-Geisser adjustment at the level of 0.05.In the original article, there was an error. At page 4, in Result part, we misused “i.e.,”, and a better way is to say all of the electrodes in the Figure 2.A correction has been made to Result, Paragraph 1: The analysis of behavior data was not performed due to the high accuracy rate (96.11% for all participants) and the fixed duration of target word presentation. Therefore, we mainly focused on ERP data analysis to identify the significant neural markers underlying the processing of emotion-label words and emotionladen words recognition. Figures 2,3 display the ERP waveforms for each condition at indicated electrodes (i.e., P7/P8, P9/P10, PO7/PO8, C1/C2, C3/C4, and C5/C6). Clearly, P100 and N170 components at the occipital-temporal sites and LPC at the central sites were identified.In the original article, there was an error. At page 6, in Discussion part, we made a wrong statement that misconnected the sentences. Original sentence stated that however, negative emotion words elicited larger LPC than positive words. But previous sentence conveyed the same message as the sentence did. Apparently, here, the “however” was misused and the sentence should be corrected, because it would confuse the readers.A correction has been made to Discussion, Paragraph 4: As for the final ERP component LPC, a late positivity (usually 500–600 ms after stimuli onset) at the central sites (Citron, 2012 ) reflecting a late and deeper elaboration of focused information (Citron, 2012 ; Chen et al., 2015 ), two main findings were revealed in the present study. First, a main effect of valence was identified that negative words generated larger LPC than positive words. The result was in line with previous findings that negative words induced larger LPC than neutral words and positive words (Bernat et al., 2001 ; Kanske and Kotz, 2007 ). However, there was some evidence that indicated positive words elicited larger LPC than negative words (Herbert et al., 2008 ; Kissler et al., 2009 ; Zhang et al., 2014 ). Zhang et al. ( 2014 ) attributed such positivity bias to “positivity offset” that was responsive to processing priority for negative words at early stages and positive words therefore enhance elaboration at later stages. By contrast, in the present study, we found negative bias on LPC. There might be two reasons to explain our results. First reason was that we not only included emotion-laden words but also emotion-label words. Possibly, a late positivity bias could be associated with large proportion of emotion-laden words in the stimuli list. After increasing the number of emotion-label words, a negative bias would probably be expected. For example, Bernat et al. ( 2001 ) found negative emotion-label words elicited larger LPC than positive emotion-label words and Bernat et al. ( 2001 ) did not contain a
机译:在原始文章中,有一个错误。在第4页上,我们写错了电极,中心位置应为C1 / C2,C3 / C4,C5 / C6,而不是CP1 / CP2,CP3 / CP4和CP5 / CP6,从而使其与本文的其他部分保持一致。对方法,EEG记录和处理,第2段进行了更正:根据目视检查的平均波形(图2),脑形貌(图3)和先前的研究(Zhang等人,2014; Chen等人)等人,2015年),我们分别在90-140、140-200和470-620的时间窗口内分析了三个明显的ERP组件,包括P100,N170和LPC。对于P100和N170,计算了沿两个半球的枕骨区域(P7 / P8,P9 / P10和PO7 / PO8)中电极的平均振幅,与先前的研究一致(Scott等,2009; Bayer等) 。,2012)。此外,分析了LPC在470-620 ms时间窗(LPC)内的中心城市(C1 / C2,C3 / P4和C5 / C6)的平均幅度。通过在0.05的水平上进行Greenhouse-Geisser调整来执行统计显着性。在原始文章中,存在错误。在第4页上,我们用文本指示了错误的图形,大脑拓扑应该是图3,而平均波形应该是图2。在文本中,我们使用了相反的引用。和处理,第2段:基于肉眼观察到的平均波形(图2),脑形貌(图3)和先前的研究(Zhang等,2014; Chen等,2015),我们进行了分析三个明显的ERP组件分别在90–140、140–200和470–620的时间范围内,包括P100,N170和LPC。对于P100和N170,计算了沿两个半球的枕骨区域(P7 / P8,P9 / P10和PO7 / PO8)中电极的平均振幅,与先前的研究一致(Scott等,2009; Bayer等) 。,2012)。此外,分析了LPC在470-620 ms时间窗(LPC)内的中心城市(C1 / C2,C3 / P4和C5 / C6)的平均幅度。通过在0.05的水平上进行Greenhouse-Geisser调整来执行统计显着性。在原始文章中,存在错误。在“结果”部分的第4页上,我们误用了“即”,更好的方法是说图2中的所有电极。已经对“结果”进行了更正,第1段:未进行行​​为数据分析归因于较高的准确率(所有参与者为96.11%)和目标词提示的固定持续时间。因此,我们主要侧重于ERP数据分析,以识别在处理情感标签词和含情感词的过程中重要的神经标记。图2,3显示了在指示电极(即P7 / P8,P9 / P10,PO7 / PO8,C1 / C2,C3 / C4和C5 / C6)上每种情况的ERP波形。显然,在枕颞部部位有P100和N170成分,在中央部位有LPC。在原始文章中有一个错误。在第6页的“讨论”部分,我们做出了一个错误的陈述,使句子之间的连接错了。原始句子指出,负面情绪词比正面情绪词引起更大的LPC。但是先前的句子传达了与句子相同的信息。显然,这里,“无论如何”被滥用,应该改正句子,因为这会使读者感到困惑。已对“讨论”第4段进行了更正:至于最后的ERP组件LPC,则是积极性较晚(通常为500-刺激发生后600毫秒(Citron,2012)反映了重点信息的后期和更深层次的阐述(Citron,2012; Chen等,2015),本研究揭示了两个主要发现。首先,确定了化合价的主要作用,即负面词比正面词产生更大的LPC。该结果与先前的发现相符,即负面词比正面词和正面词诱导更大的LPC(Bernat等,2001; Kanske和Kotz,2007)。然而,有证据表明,正面词比负面词引起更大的LPC(Herbert等,2008; Kissler等,2009; Zhang等,2014)。张等。 (2014)将这种积极性偏见归因于“积极性偏移”,它对早期阶段否定词的处理优先级做出响应,因此,积极词会在后期阶段增强阐述。相比之下,在本研究中,我们发现LPC具有负偏见。可能有两个原因可以解释我们的结果。第一个原因是我们不仅包含了充满情感的单词,还包含了带有情感标签的单词。可能的是,后期积极的偏见可能与刺激清单中大部分带有情感的单词有关。在增加带有情感标签的单词的数量之后,可能会出现负面的偏见。例如,Bernat等。 (2001)发现消极的情感标签词比积极的情感标签词引起的LPC更大,Bernat等(2001)。 (2001)不包含

著录项

相似文献

  • 外文文献
  • 中文文献
  • 专利
获取原文

客服邮箱:kefu@zhangqiaokeyan.com

京公网安备:11010802029741号 ICP备案号:京ICP备15016152号-6 六维联合信息科技 (北京) 有限公司©版权所有
  • 客服微信

  • 服务号