首页> 外文期刊>Animals >Tail Docking of Canine Puppies: Reassessment of the Tail’s Role in Communication, the Acute Pain Caused by Docking and Interpretation of Behavioural Responses
【24h】

Tail Docking of Canine Puppies: Reassessment of the Tail’s Role in Communication, the Acute Pain Caused by Docking and Interpretation of Behavioural Responses

机译:犬幼犬的尾巴对接:重新评估尾巴在交流中的作用,因对接而引起的急性疼痛和行为反应的解释

获取原文
       

摘要

Laws, regulations and professional standards increasingly aim to ban or restrict non-therapeutic tail docking in canine puppies. These constraints have usually been justified by reference to loss of tail participation in communication between dogs, the acute pain presumed to be caused during docking itself, subsequent experiences of chronic pain and heightened pain sensitivity, and the occurrence of other complications. These areas are reconsidered here. First, a scientifically robust examination of the dynamic functional foundations, sensory components and key features of body language that are integral to canine communication shows that the role of the tail has been greatly underestimated. More specifically, it shows that tail behaviour is so embedded in canine communication that docking can markedly impede unambiguous interactions between different dogs and between dogs and people. These interactions include the expression of wide ranges of both negative and positive emotions, moods and intentions that are of daily significance for dog welfare. Moreover, all docked dogs may experience these impediments throughout their lives, which challenges assertions by opponents to such bans or restrictions that the tail is a dispensable appendage. Second, and in contrast, a re-examination of the sensory capacities of canine puppies reveals that they cannot consciously experience acute or chronic pain during at least the first week after birth, which is when they are usually docked. The contrary view is based on questionable between-species extrapolation of information about pain from neurologically mature newborns such as calves, lambs, piglets and human infants, which certainly can consciously experience pain in response to injury, to neurologically immature puppies which remain unconscious and therefore unable to experience pain until about two weeks after birth. Third, underpinned by the incorrect conclusion that puppies are conscious at the usual docking age, it is argued here that the well-validated human emotional drive or desire to care for and protect vulnerable young, leads observers to misread striking docking-induced behaviour as indicating that the puppies consciously experience significant acute pain and distress. Fourth, updated information reaffirms the conclusion that a significant proportion of dogs docked as puppies will subsequently experience persistent and significant chronic pain and heightened pain sensitivity. And fifth, other reported negative consequences of docking should also be considered because, although their prevalence is unclear, when they do occur they would have significant negative welfare impacts. It is argued that the present analysis strengthens the rationale for such bans or restrictions on docking of puppies by clarifying which of several justifications previously used are and are not scientifically supportable. In particular, it highlights the major roles the tail plays in canine communication, as well as the lifetime handicaps to communication caused by docking. Thus, it is concluded that non-therapeutic tail docking of puppies represents an unnecessary removal of a necessary appendage and should therefore be banned or restricted.
机译:法律,法规和专业标准越来越多地旨在禁止或限制犬类幼犬的非治疗性对接。这些约束条件通常是通过提及狗之间交流中尾部参与的丧失,推测是在对接本身时引起的急性疼痛,随后的慢性疼痛和疼痛敏感性增强以及其他并发症的发生而证明的。这些区域在这里重新考虑。首先,科学地检查犬语言中不可或缺的动态功能基础,感觉成分和肢体语言的关键特征,表明尾巴的作用被大大低估了。更具体地说,它表明尾巴行为已深深地植入犬类交流中,以至于对接可显着阻止不同狗之间以及狗与人之间的明确交互。这些互动包括对狗的福利至关重要的各种负面和正面情绪,情绪和意图的表达。而且,所有停靠的狗在其一生中都可能遇到这些障碍,这挑战了反对者对尾巴是可有可无的附属物的禁令或限制的主张。其次,相比之下,对犬幼犬的感觉能力的重新检查表明,它们至少在出生后的第一周(即通常停靠的时候)不能有意识地经历急性或慢性疼痛。相反的观点是基于关于来自神经学成熟的新生儿(例如小牛,羊羔,小猪和人类婴儿)的疼痛信息的可疑种间推断,这些信息肯定会因受伤而自觉遭受疼痛,而对于神经学上不成熟的幼犬仍然无意识,因此直到出生后约两周才经历疼痛。第三,基于错误的结论,即幼犬在通常的对接年龄处有意识这一错误结论,在此认为,经过充分验证的人类情感驱动力或渴望照顾和保护易受伤害的年轻人的愿望,导致观察者误解了显着的对接诱发的行为,表明幼犬有意识地经历了严重的急性疼痛和痛苦。第四,更新的信息再次证实了这样的结论,即,很大一部分作为幼犬停靠的狗随后将遭受持续性和严重的慢性疼痛,并增加了疼痛敏感性。第五,还应考虑对接的其他负面影响,因为尽管尚不清楚对接的普遍性,但一旦发生,将对福利产生重大负面影响。有人认为,本分析通过阐明以前使用的几种理由中的哪些是科学上不可以支持的,从而加强了这种禁止或限制对接幼犬的理由。特别是,它突出了尾巴在犬类交往中所起的主要作用,以及由于对接而导致的终生交流障碍。因此,得出的结论是,幼犬的非治疗性尾巴对接表示不必要摘除必要的附件,因此应予以禁止或限制。

著录项

相似文献

  • 外文文献
  • 中文文献
  • 专利
获取原文

客服邮箱:kefu@zhangqiaokeyan.com

京公网安备:11010802029741号 ICP备案号:京ICP备15016152号-6 六维联合信息科技 (北京) 有限公司©版权所有
  • 客服微信

  • 服务号