首页> 外文OA文献 >PSYCHOPHYSICAL AND PSYCHOSOCIAL HUMOUR JUDGEMENTS AS A FUNCTION OF INTERACTIVE INCONGRUITY HUMOUR.
【2h】

PSYCHOPHYSICAL AND PSYCHOSOCIAL HUMOUR JUDGEMENTS AS A FUNCTION OF INTERACTIVE INCONGRUITY HUMOUR.

机译:心理和社会幽默判断作为互动非谐幽默的作用。

摘要

Based on a theoretical reconceptualization of incongruity humour theory, an interactive approach (in which humour occurs as joint function of the stimulus situation and the mental experience of the organism) is posited in this investigation. Essential theoretical properties focus on conceptual functioning, a belief--attitude distinction, syntactic versus semantic formulations of u22violation of expectancyu22, and a belief switching--belief change distinction. Mull (1949), Nerhardt (1970;1975;1976;1977), Deckers and Kizer (1974;1975), Hoppe (1976), Issar (1976), Mutuma (1976), and Tsang (1976) demonstrate the manipulation of psychophysical and psychosocial humour judgements in various experimental contexts. In the present study, five major hypotheses were experimentally tested: (1) Whether violation of belief expectancy occurs as a function of a range of expectation (Nerhardt, 1970,1975) or as a means of expectation (Deckers and Kizer, 1974,1975); (2) Whether heavy-to-light weights or light-to-heavy weights will generate contrast effect (Sherif-Hovland, 1961; Helson, 1964); (3) Whether subjects respond differentially to the violation of a narrow range of belief expectancy as opposed to the violation of a broad range of expectancy (Sherif et al., 1958; La Fave, 1977); (4) Whether subjects who are exposed frequently to beliefs respond differently from subjects who are infrequently exposed to beliefs (Sherif-Hovland, 1961; Sherif et al., 1965); and (5) Whether a heavy discrepant weight will be judged more incongruous than a light discrepant weight (Spencer, 1860; Gerber and Routh, 1975). Five dependent measures (amusement, threat, surprise, playful, and discrepant) were established for each of the five main hypotheses, generating twenty-five hypotheses in all. Hypotheses 1, 6, 11, 16, and 21 were all substantiated (p u3c .01): conditions in which range was varied were more amusing, less threatening, more surprising, more playful, and more discrepant than conditions in which mean was varied, lending support to Nerhardtu27s (1970;1975) findings in the context of a Sherif-Hovland social-judgement framework. Hypotheses 2, 7, 12, 17, and 22 failed to be substantiated: no contrast effects between the end-of-series weights and the discrepant weights occurred for either amusement, threat, surprise, playfulness, or discrepancy. The violation of a narrow range of belief expectancy generated significantly (p u3c .01) more amusement and playfulness (p u3c .05) for subjects than the violation of a broad range of belief expectancy, supporting hypotheses 3 and 8. A MANOVA did significantly (p u3c .01) provide support for hypotheses 3, 8, 13, 18, and 23. Subjects, who were frequently exposed to the initial series of weights, judged the same discrepant weight as significantly (p u3c .01) more amusing, surprising, playful, and discrepant than subjects who were infrequently exposed to the initial series of weights (substantiation for hypotheses 4, 9, 19, and 24). A MANOVA was significant (p u3c .05) for these frequency of exposure hypotheses (4, 9, 14, 19, and 24). As Sherif et al. (1958) suggest, subjectsu27 responses were found to be more intense, when their level of commitment or degree of ego-involvement is aroused. The findings in this study demonstrate that it is possible to exact a similar effect with beliefs. For the last set of hypotheses (5,10, 15, 20, and 25), a discrepant weight, heavier than any of the other weights previously lifted, was judged to be significantly (p u3c .05) more playful and surprising than a light discrepant weight (hypotheses 10 and 20). The MANOVA, for this direction of discrepant weight hypothesis, was insignificant (p u3c .10). Interpretation of this data suggests that a quantification of Spenceru27s ascending/descending incongruity distinction may be illegitimate. This aspect of incongruity humour remains relatively unexplored, and further research on this important topic is suggested.Dept. of Psychology. Paper copy at Leddy Library: Theses u26 Major Papers - Basement, West Bldg. / Call Number: Thesis1980 .G944. Source: Dissertation Abstracts International, Volume: 41-10, Section: B, page: 3933. Thesis (Ph.D.)--University of Windsor (Canada), 1980.
机译:基于不协调幽默理论的理论重新概念化,本研究提出了一种互动方法(其中幽默是刺激情况和机体心理经验的联合功能)。基本的理论特性集中在概念功能,信念-态度区别,期望的违反的句法与语义表述以及信念转换-信念变化的区别上。 Mull(1949),Nerhardt(1970; 1975; 1976; 1977),Deckers和Kizer(1974; 1975),Hoppe(1976),Issar(1976),Mutuma(1976)和Tsang(1976)证明了对心理物理学的操纵和在各种实验情况下的社会心理幽默判断。在本研究中,对五个主要假设进行了实验检验:(1)违反信念期望的发生是期望范围的函数(Nerhardt,1970,1975)还是期望的手段(Deckers和Kizer,1974,1975) ); (2)无论是重到轻还是轻到重都会产生对比效果(Sherif-Hovland,1961; Helson,1964); (3)受试者是否对窄范围的预期期望的违反与对宽范围的期望的违反有不同的反应(Sherif et al。,1958; La Fave,1977); (4)经常接触信念的受试者的反应与不经常接触信念的受试者的反应是否不同(Sherif-Hovland,1961; Sherif等,1965); (5)判断较重的重量是否比较轻的重量更不一致(Spencer,1860; Gerber and Routh,1975)。为这五个主要假设中的每一个建立了五种相关的度量(娱乐,威胁,惊奇,嬉戏和差异),总共产生了二十五个假设。假设1、6、11、16和21都得到了证实(p u3c .01):范围变化的条件比均值的条件更有趣,更不具威胁性,更令人惊讶,更嬉戏并且差异更大。在Sherif-Hovland社会判断框架的背景下,对Nerhardt u27s(1970; 1975)的发现提供了各种支持。假设2、7、12、17和22无法得到证实:对于娱乐性,威胁性,惊奇性,嬉戏性或差异性,系列末期权重和差异权重之间没有形成对比作用。违反狭义的信念期望范围比宽泛的信念期望范围产生了更多的娱乐性和嬉戏性(p u3c.05),这支持了假设3和8。确实有显着性(p u3c .01)为假设3、8、13、18和23提供了支持。经常暴露于初始权重系列的受试者认为相同的体重差异显着(p u3c.01)。 )比那些不经常接触初始权重系列(假设4、9、19和24的证据)的受试者更有趣,令人惊讶,嬉戏和差异更大。对于这些暴露假说的频率(4、9、14、19和24),MANOVA显着(p <0.05)。如Sherif等。 (1958)建议,当激发他们的承诺水平或自我参与程度时,发现他们的反应更加强烈。这项研究的发现表明,有可能在信念上产生相似的效果。对于最后一组假设(5、10、15、20和25),被认为比以前举起的其他任何重量都重的重量相差很大(p u3c .05),比轻巧的重量(假设10和20)。对于差异体重假设的这个方向,MANOVA无关紧要(p u3c .10)。对该数据的解释表明,对Spencer升/降不一致性的量化量化可能是非法的。不协调幽默的这一方面仍未得到充分探索,建议对该重要主题进行进一步研究。心理学。莱迪图书馆的纸质副本:论文主要论文-西楼地下室。 /电话号码:Thesis1980 .G944。资料来源:国际学位论文摘要,第41-10卷,第B部分,第3933页。论文(博士学位)-温莎大学(加拿大),1980年。

著录项

相似文献

  • 外文文献
  • 中文文献
  • 专利

客服邮箱:kefu@zhangqiaokeyan.com

京公网安备:11010802029741号 ICP备案号:京ICP备15016152号-6 六维联合信息科技 (北京) 有限公司©版权所有
  • 客服微信

  • 服务号